Английская Википедия:Central Alaskan Yupʼik

Материал из Онлайн справочника
Версия от 05:15, 16 февраля 2024; EducationBot (обсуждение | вклад) (Новая страница: «{{Английская Википедия/Панель перехода}} {{Redirect-distinguish2|Yup'ik language|the Yupik languages}} {{short description|Language of the Yupik family, spoken in western and southwestern Alaska}} {{Infobox language | name = Central Alaskan Yupik | altname = Yupʼik | nativename = ''Yugtun, Cugtun'' | states = United States | region = western and southwestern Alaska | ethnicity = Yup'ik|Central Alaskan Yupik peo...»)
(разн.) ← Предыдущая версия | Текущая версия (разн.) | Следующая версия → (разн.)
Перейти к навигацииПерейти к поиску

Шаблон:Redirect-distinguish2 Шаблон:Short description Шаблон:Infobox language Central Alaskan Yupʼik (also rendered Yupik,[1] Central Yupik,[2][3] or indigenously Yugtun) is one of the languages of the Yupik family, in turn a member of the Eskimo–Aleut language group, spoken in western and southwestern Alaska. Both in ethnic population and in number of speakers, the Central Alaskan Yupik people form the largest group among Alaska Natives. As of 2010 Yupʼik was, after Navajo, the second most spoken aboriginal language in the United States.[4] Yupʼik should not be confused with the related language Central Siberian Yupik spoken in Chukotka and St. Lawrence Island, nor Naukan Yupik likewise spoken in Chukotka.

Yupʼik, like all Eskimo languages, is polysynthetic and uses suffixation as primary means for word formation. There are a great number of derivational suffixes (termed postbases) that are used productively to form these polysynthetic words. Yupʼik has predominantly ergative alignment: case marking follows the ergative pattern for the most part, but verb agreement can follow an ergative or an accusative pattern, depending on grammatical mood.[5] The language grammatically distinguishes three numbers: singular, dual, and plural. There is no marking of grammatical gender in the language, nor are there articles.

Language name

The Yup'ik language goes by various names. Since it is a geographically central member of the Yupik languages and is spoken in Alaska, the language is often referred to as Central Alaskan Yupik (for example, in Miyaoka's 2012 grammar of the language). The term Yup'ik [jupːik] is a common endonym, and is derived from /juɣ-piɣ/ "person-genuine".[5] The Alaska Native Language Center and Jacobson's (1995) learner's grammar use Central (Alaskan) Yup'ik, which can be seen as a hybrid of the former two terms; there is, however, potential for confusion here: Central (Alaskan) Yup'ik may refer to either the language as a whole, or the geographically central dialect of the language, more commonly called General Central Yup'ik.

Other endonyms are used regionally: Cup'ig in the Nunivak dialect, Cup'ik in Chevak (these terms are cognate with Yup'ik, but represent the pronunciation of the word in the respective dialect), and Yugtun in the Yukon-Kuskokwim region.

Geographic distribution and use

Yupʼik is spoken primarily in southwestern Alaska, from Norton Sound in the north to the Alaska Peninsula in the south, and from Lake Iliamna in the east to Nunivak Island in the west. Yup'ik lies geographically central relative to the other members of the Yupik language family: Alutiiq ~ Sugpiaq is spoken to south and east, and Central Siberian Yupik is spoken to the west on St. Lawrence Island (often called St. Lawrence Island Yupik in the Alaskan context) and on the Chukotka peninsula, where Naukan Yupik is also spoken. Yup'ik is bordered to the north by the more distantly related Iñupiaq language; the difference between Yupʼik and Iñupiaq is comparable to that of the difference between Spanish and French.[6]

Of a total population of more than 23,000 people, more than 14,000 are speakers of the language.[7] Children still grow up speaking Yupʼik as their first language in 17 of 68 Yupʼik villages, those mainly located on the lower Kuskokwim River, on Nelson Island, and along the coast between the Kuskokwim River and Nelson Island. The variety of Yup'ik spoken by the younger generations is being influenced strongly by English: it is less synthetic, has a reduced inventory of spatial demonstratives, and is lexically Anglicized.[5]

Dialects

Yup'ik is typically considered to have five dialects: Norton Sound, General Central Yup'ik, Nunivak, Hooper Bay-Chevak, and the extinct Egegik dialect.[5][8][9] All extant dialects of the language are mutually intelligible, albeit with phonological and lexical differences that sometimes cause difficulty in cross-dialectal comprehension.[8][10] Lexical differences exist somewhat dramatically across dialects, in part due to a historical practice of name taboo.[5] Speakers may be reluctant to take on the lexicon of another dialect because they "often feel proud of their own dialects".[8]

The Yupʼik dialects, sub-dialects and their locations are as follows:[11]

  1. Norton Sound (alternatively, Unaliq-Pastuliq); spoken around Norton Sound
    1. Unaliq sub-dialect; spoken by Unalirmiut (= Atnegmiut, Kuuyuŋmiut, Eŋlutaleġmiut etc.) tribes in Elim (Neviarcaurluq), Golovin (Cingik), St. Michael (Taciq)
    2. Kotlik sub-dialect; spoken by the Pastulirmiut tribe in Kotlik (Qerrulliik)
  2. General Central Yupʼik (Yugtun); spoken on Nelson Island, on the Yukon-Kuskokwim delta, and in the Bristol Bay region of Alaska[8]
    1. Core dialects; spoken on the lower Kuskokwim, on the coast up to Nelson Island, and in Bristol Bay[9]
      1. Lower Kuskokwim sub-dialect; spoken in Akiachak (Akiacuaq), Akiak (Akiaq), Atmautluak (Atmaulluaq), Bethel (Mamterilleq), Eek (Ekvicuaq), Goodnews Bay (Mamterat), Upper Kalskag (Qalqaq), Lower Kalskag (Qalqaq), Kipnuk (Qipnek), Kongiganak (Kangirnaq), Kwethluk (Kuiggluk), Kwigillingok (Kuigilnguq), Napakiak (Naparyarraq), Napaskiak (Napaskiaq), Nunapitchuk or Akolmiut (Nunapicuar), Oscarville (Kuiggayagaq), Platinum (Arviiq), Quinhagak (Kuinerraq), Tuluksak (Tuulkessaaq), and Tuntutuliak (Tuntutuliaq)
      2. Bristol Bay sub-dialect; spoken in Aleknagik (Alaqnaqiq), Clark's Point (Saguyaq), Dillingham (Curyung), Ekuk, Manokotak (Manuquutaq), Togiak (Tuyuryaq), and Twin Hills (Ingricuar)
    2. Peripheral dialects; spoken on the upper Kuskokwim, on the Yukon, and around Lake Iliamna[9]
      1. Yukon or Lower Yukon sub-dialect; spoken in Alakanuk (Alarneq), Emmonak (Imangaq), Holy Cross (Ingirraller), Marshall or Fortuna Ledge (Masserculleq), Mountain Village (Asaacaryaraq), Nunam Iqua or Sheldon Point (Nunam Iqua), Pilot Station (Tuutalgaq), Pitkas Point (Negeqliim Painga), Russian Mission (Iqugmiut), St. Mary's (Negeqliq), Scammon Bay (Marayaarmiut)
      2. Upper or Middle Kuskokwim sub-dialect; spoken in Aniak (Anyaraq), Chuathbaluk (Curarpalek), Crooked Creek (Qipcarpak), McGrath, Sleetmute (Cellitemiut), Stony River
      3. Lake Iliamna sub-dialect; spoken in Egegik (Igyagiiq), Igiugig (Igyaraq), Iliamna (Illiamna), Kokhanok (Qarrʼunaq), Levelock (Liivlek), Naknek (Nakniq), South Naknek (Qinuyang)
    3. Mixed dialects (i.e., those that share features of core and peripheral varieties)
      1. Nelson Island & Stebbins sub-dialect; spoken in Chefornak (Cevvʼarneq), Newtok (Niugtaq), Nightmute (Negtemiut), Stebbins (Tapraq), Toksook Bay (Nunakauyaq or Qaluuyarmiut), Tununak (Tununeq)
      2. Nushagak River sub-dialect; spoken in Ekwok (Iquaq), Koliganek (Qalirneq), New Stuyahok (Cetuyaraq), Portage Creek
  3. Egegik (extinct), once spoken in Egegik (Igyagiiq)
  4. Hooper Bay-Chevak
    1. Hooper Bay sub-dialect; spoken in Hooper Bay (Naparyaarmiut)
    2. Chevak sub-dialect; spoken in Chevak (Cevʼaq),
  5. Nunivak; spoken in Mekoryuk (Mikuryar).

The last of these, the Nunivak dialect (Cupʼig) is distinct and highly divergent from mainland Yupʼik dialects. The only significant difference between Hooper Bay and Chevak dialects is the pronunciation of the initial y- [j] as c- [tʃ] in Chevak in some words: Yupʼik in Hooper Bay but Cupʼik in Chevak.[5]

Even sub-dialects may differ with regard to pronunciation and lexicon. The following table compares some words in two sub-dialects of General Central Yupʼik (Yugtun).[12]

Yukon (Kuigpak) Kuskokwim (Kusquqvak) meaning
Шаблон:Lang Шаблон:Lang to study (intrans.); to teach someone (trans.)
Шаблон:Lang Шаблон:Lang student
Шаблон:Lang Шаблон:Lang to teach (intrans.)
Шаблон:Lang Шаблон:Lang teacher
Шаблон:Lang Шаблон:Lang hand
Шаблон:Lang Шаблон:Lang elbow
Шаблон:Lang Шаблон:Lang to cook by boiling
Шаблон:Lang Шаблон:Lang weather, outside, universe, awareness
Шаблон:Lang Шаблон:Lang lamp, light
Шаблон:Lang Шаблон:Lang to sample or taste, attempt, try

Writing and literature

A syllabary known as the Yugtun script was invented for the language by Uyaquq, a native speaker, in about 1900, although the language is now mostly written using the Latin script.[13] Early linguistic work in Central Yupʼik was done primarily by Russian Orthodox, then Jesuit and Moravian Church missionaries, leading to a modest tradition of literacy used in letter writing. In the 1960s, Irene Reed and others at the Alaska Native Language Center developed a modern writing system for the language. Their work led to the establishment of the state's first bilingual school programs in four Yupʼik villages in the early 1970s. Since then a wide variety of bilingual materials has been published, including Steven Jacobson's comprehensive dictionary of the language, his complete practical classroom grammar, and story collections and narratives by many others including a full novel by Anna Jacobson.

Orthography

Файл:PLEDGE OF ALLEGIANCE in YUPIK (2840468808).jpg
The Pledge of Allegiance in Yupʼik. This uses a variant orthography with <gh> instead of <r> to indicate the voiced uvular fricative.

While several different systems have been used to write Yupʼik, the most widely used orthography today is that adopted by the Alaska Native Language Center and exemplified in Jacobson's (1984) dictionary, Jacobson's (1995) learner's grammar, and Miyaoka's (2012) grammar. The orthography is a Latin-script alphabet; the letters and digraphs used in alphabetical order are listed below, along with an indication of their associated phonemes in the International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA).

Letter / digraph IPA Letter / digraph IPA Notes
a /a/ p /p/
c /tʃ/ q /q/
e /ə/ r /ʁ/ represents /χ/ word-finally
g /ɣ/ rr /χ/
gg /x/ s /z/ represents /s/ word-initially
i /i/ ss /s/
k /k/ t /t/
l /l/ u /u/
ll /ɬ/ u͡g /ɣʷ/
m /m/ u͡r /ʁʷ/
ḿ /m̥/ u͡rr [χʷ] does not contrast with /ʁʷ/
n /n/ v /v/
ń /n̥/ vv /f/
ng /ŋ/ w /xʷ/
ńg /ŋ̊/ y /j/

The vowel qualities /a, i, u/ may occur long; these are written aa, ii, uu when vowel length is not a result of stress. Consonants may also occur long (geminate), but their occurrence is often predictable by regular phonological rules, and so in these cases is not marked in the orthography. Where long consonants occur unpredictably they are indicated with an apostrophe following consonant. For example, Yupiaq and Yupʼik both contain a geminate p (/pː/). In Yupiaq length is predictable and hence is not marked; in Yupʼik the length is not predictable and so must be indicated with the apostrophe. An apostrophe is also used to separate n from g, to distinguish n'g /nɣ/ from the digraph ng /ŋ/. Apostrophes are also used between two consonants to indicate that voicing assimilation has not occurred (see below), and between two vowels to indicate the lack of gemination of a preceding consonant. A hyphen is used to separate a clitic from its host.

Phonology

Vowels

Yup'ik contrasts four vowel qualities: Шаблон:IPA. The reduced vowel Шаблон:IPA always manifests phonetically short in duration, but the other three vowel qualities may occur phonetically short or long: Шаблон:IPA. Phonetically long vowels come about when a full vowel (Шаблон:IPA) is lengthened by stress (see below), or when two single vowels are brought together across a morpheme boundary. The effect is that while phonetic vowel length may yield a surface contrast between words, phonetic length is predictable and thus not phonemically contrastive.[5]

Yup'ik vowel phonemes
Front (unrounded) Central

(unrounded)

Back

rounded

Non-low i ə u
Low a

The vowel qualities Шаблон:IPA are allophones of Шаблон:IPA, and are found preceding uvular consonants (such as Шаблон:IPA or Шаблон:IPA) and preceding the low vowel Шаблон:IPA.[5]

Consonants

Yup'ik does not contrast voicing in stops, but has a wide range of fricatives that contrast in voicing. The phoneme Шаблон:IPA is not phonetically a fricative, but behaves as one phonologically in Yup'ik (in particular with regard to voicing alternations, where it alternates with Шаблон:IPAblink; see below). Contrasts between Шаблон:IPA and Шаблон:IPA and between Шаблон:IPA and Шаблон:IPA are rare, and the greater part of the voicing contrasts among fricatives is between the laterals Шаблон:IPA and Шаблон:IPA, the velars Шаблон:IPA and Шаблон:IPA, and the uvulars Шаблон:IPA and Шаблон:IPA. For some speakers, there is also a voicing contrast among the nasal consonants, which is typologically somewhat rare. Any consonant may occur as a geminate word-medially, and consonant length is contrastive.[5]

Yup'ik consonants
Labial Alveolar Postalv./
Palatal
Velar Uvular
Шаблон:Small Шаблон:Small Шаблон:Small Шаблон:Small
Nasal Шаблон:Small Шаблон:IPAlink Шаблон:IPAlink Шаблон:IPAlink
Шаблон:Small Шаблон:IPAlink Шаблон:IPAlink Шаблон:IPAlink
Stop Шаблон:IPAlink Шаблон:IPAlink Шаблон:IPAlink Шаблон:IPAlink
Affricate (t͡s) t͡ʃ
Fricative Шаблон:Small Шаблон:IPAlink Шаблон:IPAlink Шаблон:IPAlink Шаблон:IPAlink (χʷ)
Шаблон:Small Шаблон:IPAlink Шаблон:IPAlink Шаблон:IPAlink ɣʷ Шаблон:IPAlink ʁʷ
lateral Шаблон:IPAlink
Approximant Шаблон:IPAlink Шаблон:IPAlink (w)

The table above includes the allophones Шаблон:IPA, Шаблон:IPA, and Шаблон:IPA. The voiceless labialized uvular fricative Шаблон:IPA occurs only in some speech variants and does not contrast with its voiced counterpart Шаблон:IPA. The voiceless alveolar affricate Шаблон:IPA is an allophone of Шаблон:IPA before the schwa vowel. The voiced labiovelar approximant Шаблон:IPA is an allophone of Шаблон:IPA that typically occurs between two full vowels, excepting when it occurs adjacent to an inflectional suffix. For example, Шаблон:IPA "work-place-ABS" is pronounced Шаблон:IPA (orthographically, calivik), since Шаблон:IPA occurs between two full vowels and it not adjacent to the inflectional suffix. With Шаблон:IPA "oar" by contrast, since Шаблон:IPA is an inflectional suffix, Шаблон:IPA does not undergo the allophonic alternation: Шаблон:IPA (cavun).[5]

Dialect variations

In Norton Sound, as well as some villages on the lower Yukon, Шаблон:IPA tends to be pronounced as Шаблон:IPA when following a consonant, and geminate Шаблон:IPA as Шаблон:IPA. For example, the word angyaq "boat" of General Central Yup'ik (GCY) is angsaq Шаблон:IPA Norton Sound.[9][5]

Conversely, in the Hooper Bay-Chevak (HBC) dialect, there is no Шаблон:IPA phoneme, and Шаблон:IPA is used in its place, such that GCY qasgiq Шаблон:IPA is pronounced qaygiq Шаблон:IPA. HBC does not have the Шаблон:IPA allophone of Шаблон:IPA, such that Шаблон:IPA is pronounced Шаблон:IPA in all contexts,[9] and there are no labialized uvular fricatives.[5]

In the Nunivak dialect, one finds Шаблон:IPA in place of GCY Шаблон:IPA, such that GCY cukaitut "they are slow" is pronounced cukaatut, there is no word-final fortition of Шаблон:IPA and Шаблон:IPA (see below), and word-initial Шаблон:IPA is pronounced Шаблон:IPA.[9]

Voicing alternations

There are a variety of voicing assimilation processes (specifically, devoicing) that apply mostly predictably to continuant consonants (fricatives and nasals); these processes are not represented in the orthography.[9]

Occasionally these assimilation processes do not apply, and in the orthography an apostrophe is written in the middle of the consonant cluster to indicate this: at'nguq is pronounced Шаблон:IPA, not Шаблон:IPA.[9]

Fricatives are devoiced word-initially and word-finally.[5]

Word-final fortition

Another common phonological alternation of Yup'ik is word-final fortition. Among consonants, only the stops Шаблон:IPA, the nasals Шаблон:IPA, and the fricative Шаблон:IPA may occur word-finally. Any other fricative (and in many cases also Шаблон:IPA) will become a plosive when it occurs at the end of a word. For example, qayar-pak "big kayak" is pronounced Шаблон:IPA, while "kayak" alone is Шаблон:IPA; the velar fricative becomes a stop word-finally. Moreover, the Шаблон:IPA of -pak is only a stop by virtue of it being word-final: if another suffix is added, as in qayar-pag-tun "like a big kayak" a fricative is found in place of that stop: Шаблон:IPA.[5]

Elision

The voiced velar consonants Шаблон:IPA are elided between single vowels, if the first is a full vowel: Шаблон:IPA is pronounced tumai Шаблон:IPA (with geminate Шаблон:IPA resulting from automatic gemination; see below).[5]

Prosody

Yup'ik has an iambic stress system. Starting from the leftmost syllable in a word and moving rightward, syllables usually are grouped into units (termed "feet") containing two syllables each, and the second syllable of each foot is stressed. (However, feet in Yup'ik may also consist of a single syllable, which is almost always closed and must bear stress.) For example, in the word pissuqatalliniluni "apparently about to hunt", every second syllable (save the last) is stressed. The most prominent of these (i.e., the syllable that has primary stress) is the rightmost of the stressed syllables.[5]

The iambic stress system of Yup'ik results in predicable iambic lengthening, a processes that serves to increase the weight of the prominent syllable in a foot.[14] When lengthening cannot apply, a variety of processes involving either elision or gemination apply to create a well-formed prosodic word.[9][5]

Iambic lengthening

Iambic lengthening is the process by which the second syllable in an iambic foot is made more prominent by lengthening the duration of the vowel in that syllable.[14] In Yup'ik, a bisyllabic foot whose syllables each contain one phonologically single vowel will be pronounced with a long vowel in the second syllable. Thus pissuqatalliniluni Шаблон:IPA "apparently about to hunt" is pronounced Шаблон:IPA. Following standard linguistic convention, parentheses here demarcate feet, periods represent the remaining syllable boundaries, and apostrophes occur before syllables that bear stress. In this word the second, fourth, and sixth syllables are pronounced with long vowels as a result of iambic lengthening.[14][15][16] Iambic lengthening does not apply to final syllables in a word.[9][5]

Because the vowel Шаблон:IPA cannot occur long in Yup'ik, when a syllable whose nucleus is Шаблон:IPA is in line to receive stress, iambic lengthening cannot apply. Instead, one of two things may happen. In Norton Sound dialects, the consonant following Шаблон:IPA will geminate if that consonant is not part of a cluster. This also occurs outside of Norton Sound if the consonants before and after Шаблон:IPA are phonetically similar. For example, Шаблон:IPA "on the footprint" is not pronounced *Шаблон:IPA, which would be expected by iambic lengthening, but rather is pronounced Шаблон:IPA, with gemination of the second Шаблон:IPA to increase the weight of the second syllable.[14][15][16]

Regressive stress

There are a variety of prosodic factors that cause stress to retract (move backward) to a syllable where it would not otherwise be expected, given the usual iambic stress pattern. (These processes do not apply, however, in the Norton Sound dialects.[5]) The processes by which stress retracts under prosodically-conditioned factors are said to feature regression of stress in Miyaoka's (2012) grammar. When regression occurs, the syllable to which stress regresses constitutes a monosyllabic foot.[5]

The first of these processes is related to the inability of Шаблон:IPA to occur long. Outside of Norton Sound, if the consonants before and after Шаблон:IPA are phonetically dissimilar, Шаблон:IPA will elide, and stress will retract to a syllable whose nucleus is the vowel before the elided Шаблон:IPA. For example, Шаблон:IPA "his own fish" is not pronounced *Шаблон:IPA, which would be expected by iambic lengthening, but rather is pronounced neq'ni Шаблон:IPA, which features the elision of Шаблон:IPA and a monosyllabic foot.[5]

Second, if the first syllable of a word is closed (ends in a consonant), this syllable constitutes a monosyllabic foot and receives stress. Iambic footing continues left-to-right from the right edge of that foot. For example, nerciqsugnarquq "(s)he probably will eat" has the stress pattern Шаблон:IPA, with stress on the first and third syllables.[9][5]

Another third prosodic factor that influences regressive is hiatus: the occurrence of adjacent vowels. Yup'ik disallows hiatus at the boundaries between feet: any two consecutive vowels must be grouped within the same foot. If two vowels are adjacent, and the first of these would be at the right edge of a foot (and thus stressed) given the usual iambic footing, the stress retracts to a preceding syllable. Without regressive accent, Yupiaq Шаблон:IPA would be pronounced *Шаблон:IPA, but because of the ban on hiatus at foot boundaries, stress retracts to the initial syllable, and consonant gemination occurs to increase the weight of that initial syllable, resulting in Шаблон:IPA.[5] This process is termed automatic gemination in Jacobson's (1995) grammar.

Yup'ik also disallows iambic feet that consist of a closed syllable followed by an open one, i.e. feet of the form CVC.'CV(ː), where C and V stand for "consonant" and "vowel" respectively. To avoid this type of foot, stress retracts: cangatenrituten Шаблон:IPA has the stress pattern Шаблон:IPA to avoid the iambic foot *(tən.'ʁiː) that would otherwise be expected.[5]

Grammar

Yup'ik has highly synthetic morphology: the number of morphemes within a word is very high. The language is moreover agglutinative, meaning that affixation is the primary strategy for word formation, and that an affix, when added to a word, does not unpredictably affect the forms of neighboring affixes. Because of the tendency to create very long verbs through suffixation, a Yupʼik verb often carries as much information as an English sentence, and word order is often quite free.

Three parts of speech are identified: nouns, verbs, and particles. Because there are fewer parts of speech than in (e.g.) English, each category has a wider range of uses. For example, Yup'ik grammatical case fulfills the role that English prepositions do, and nominal derivational affixes or roots fulfill the role that English adjectives do.[5]

Morphology

In descriptive work on Yup'ik, there are four regions within nouns and verbs that are commonly identified. The first of these is often called the stem (equivalent to the notion of a root), which carries the core meaning of the word. Following the stem come zero or more postbases,[9] which are derivational modifiers that change the category of the word or augment its meaning. (Yup'ik does not have adjectives; nominal roots and postbases are used instead.) The third section is called an ending, which carries the inflectional categories of case (on nouns), grammatical mood (on verbs), person, and number.[17] Finally, optional enclitics may be added, which usually indicate "the speaker's attitude towards what he is saying such as questioning, hoping, reporting, etc."[17] Orthographically, enclitics are separated from the rest of the word with a hyphen.[9][5] However, since hyphens are already used in glosses to separate morphemes, there is potential for confusion as to whether a morpheme is a suffix or an enclitic, so in glosses the equals sign is used instead.

Example verb: angyarpaliyukapigtellruunga [5]
Stem Postbases Ending Enclitic
Шаблон:Interlinear Шаблон:Interlinear Шаблон:Interlinear
"I wished very much to build a big boat"
Example verb: assirtua-gguq [5]
Stem Postbases Ending Enclitic
Шаблон:Interlinear Шаблон:Interlinear Шаблон:Interlinear
"(he says) I'm fine"
"(tell him) I'm fine"
Example noun: kipusvik [9]
Stem Postbases Ending Enclitic
Шаблон:Interlinear Шаблон:Interlinear Шаблон:Interlinear
"store" (lit. "place for buying")
Example noun: qayarpaliyaraqa [5]
Stem Postbases Ending Enclitic
Шаблон:Interlinear Шаблон:Interlinear Шаблон:Interlinear
"the way I make a big kayak"

Because post-bases are derivational morphemes, and thus can change the part of speech of a word, many verbs are built from noun stems, and vice versa. For example, Шаблон:Lang "I have fish" is a verb, despite the fact that Шаблон:Lang "fish" is a noun; the postbase Шаблон:Lang "have" makes the resulting word a verb. These changes in grammatical category can apply iteratively, such that over the course of word formation, a word may become a noun, then a verb, then back to a noun, and so on.[5][18]

Verb conjugation

The conjugation of Yup'ik verbs involves obligatory marking of grammatical mood and agreement.

Grammatical mood

Yup'ik has a great number of grammatical moods. The moods can be categorized according to whether the clause in which they are found is independent or subordinate. There are four so-called independent moods: the indicative, optative, interrogative, and participial; these typically are found on the main verbs of independent clauses. Yupʼik also has ten connective moods, which occur on the verbs of adverbial clauses; the connective moods are the Yup'ik equivalent of many subordinating conjunctions of English, and are often translated as 'because', 'although', 'if' and 'while'.[19] The form of a given mood may depend on the transitivity of the verb (e.g., the intransitive form of the participial mood suffix is usually -lriar, but when this mood is suffixed to a transitive verb, its form is -ke instead),[5] on the person of the grammatical subject (e.g., the optative mood is marked with -li only if the subject is third person), or on the phonological or morphological environment.

Yup'ik grammatical moods
Forms Common uses (not exhaustive)
Independent moods Indicative -gur (intransitive)

-gar (transitive)

Used to form declarative sentences
Participial -lriar, or -ngur after /t/ (intransitive)

-ke (transitive)

(Varied)
Interrogative -ta (after a consonant, if subject is third person)

-ga (after a vowel, if subject is third person)

-ci (if subject is first or second person)

Used to form wh-questions
Optative -li (if subject is third person)

-la (if subject is first person)

-gi or -na (if subject is second person)

Used to express wishes, requests, suggestions, commands,

and occasionally to make declarative statements

Co-subordinate Appositional -lu, or -na after certain suffixes Used for cosubordination, coordination, and in independent clauses
Connective moods Causal -nga Used to form subordinate clauses (translated "because, when")
Constative -gaq(a) Used to form subordinate clauses (translated "whenever")
Precessive -pailg Used to form subordinate clauses (translated "before")
Concessive -ngrrarr Used to form subordinate clauses (translated "although, even if")
Conditional -k(u) Used to form subordinate clauses (translated "if")
Indirective -cu(a) Used to express indirect suggestions, admonishment
Contemporative -llr Used to form subordinate clauses (translated "when")
Simultaneous -nginanrr Used to form subordinate clauses (translated "while")
Stative Used to form subordinate clauses (translated "being in the state of")

In addition to the connective moods listed above, there are five so-called "quasi-connective" moods. Though these are adverbial adjuncts to main clauses and thus are similar in function to the connective moods, they inflect like nominals (they inflect with case, not agreement).[5]

Agreement

Yup'ik has a rich system of agreement on verbs. Up to two nominal arguments may be cross-referenced (intransitive verbs agree with their sole argument, and transitive verbs agree with both arguments). Three numbers (singular, dual, and plural) are distinguished, as well as at least three persons (first, second, and third). The third person is unmarked when cross-referencing subjects,[5] and the verbs of dependent clauses may have two types of third person forms depending on whether some argument is co-refers with the subject of the verb in the independent clause (see "Co-reference across clauses" below).[9][5] To the extent that subject and object agreement markers are not fusional, subject agreement linearly precedes object agreement.

Depending on the grammatical mood of the verb and which grammatical persons are being cross-referenced, agreement may display either an ergative pattern (where the sole argument of an intransitive verb is cross-referenced with the same morpheme that it would be if it were the object of a transitive verb) or an accusative pattern (where the sole argument of an intransitive verb is cross-referenced with the same morpheme that it would be if it were the subject of a transitive verb).[5]

Agreement markers vary in form depending on the grammatical mood of the verb. The two examples below illustrate this. In (1), the 1SG>3SG agreement marker is -qa because the verb is in the indicative mood, while in (2) the agreement marker is -ku due to verb being in the optative mood.

Шаблон:Interlinear

Шаблон:Interlinear

The participial and indicative share a set of agreement markers, and all the connective moods likewise share a common set (which is shared also with some possessed nouns).[5]

Co-reference across clauses

The form of 3rd-person agreement in dependent clauses may vary depending on whether that 3rd-person argument is the same referent as, or a different referent than, a 3rd-person subject of the independent clause. In some descriptive work on the language, when the subject of the independent clause is co-referential with the relevant argument in the dependent clause, the agreement in the dependent clause is said to reflect a "fourth"[9] or a "reflexive third"[5] person. Jacobson (1995) uses the following contrast to illustrate:

Шаблон:Interlinear

Шаблон:Interlinear

The intransitive agreement in the dependent clause ermig-pailg-an in (3) is -an, indicating that the argument of the dependent clause is a different referent than the subject of the independent clause nerellruuq, while in (4) the agreement -mi indicates that the arguments of each clause are co-referential. Some grammatical moods do not have associated agreement markers that contrast these two types of third person.[5]

Some researchers have argued that the contrast in (3-4) exemplifies a type of switch-reference,[20][21] though McKenzie (2015) claims Yup'ik does not have the true characteristics of switch-reference, and that the Yup'ik system is better understood in terms of obviation or long-distance anaphora.[22]

Nouns

Yup'ik nouns inflect for number, case, and show agreement with the person and number of a possessor if present.[5]

Grammatical case

The morphosyntactic alignment of Yupʼik is ergative-absolutive,[5] meaning that subjects of intransitive verbs bear the same grammatical case (the absolutive) as the objects of transitive verbs, while the subjects of transitive verbs have a different case (the ergative). For example, the sentence Angyaq tak'uq ("The boat is long") features an intransitive verb, and the subject (angyaq, "the boat") is in the absolutive case. By comparison, in the sentence Angyaq kiputaa ("He buys the boat"), the verb is transitive, and it is now the object (angyaq, "the boat") that bears the absolutive.[23] This contrasts with nominative-accusative languages like English, where the subjects of intransitives and transitives are identical in form ("He slept", "He ate the bread"), while the objects of transitives have a different case ("The moose saw him").

In addition to the absolutive and ergative structural cases (the latter of which is syncretic with the genitive; collectively the ergative and genitive are usually called the relative case[9][5]), there are at least five other cases that are mostly-nonstructural: ablative-modalis (a historical syncretism of ablative and instrumental cases), allative, locative, perlative, and equalis.[5]

Yup'ik grammatical cases
Common function(s) English equivalents
Structural Absolutive Identifies the sole argument of an intransitive verb

Identifies the definite object of a transitive verb

(none)
Relative Ergative Identifies the subject of a transitive verb (none)
Genitive Identifies a possessor 's (as in John's book)
Non-structural Ablative-modalis Identifies a spatial or temporal starting point

Marks nominals demoted from absolutive case under valency reduction

from

(none)

Allative Identifies a spatial or temporal end point

Marks nominals demoted from relative case under valency reduction

to

(none)

Locative Identifies spatial or temporal locations

Indicates the standard of comparison in comparatives

at, in, during

than

Perlative Identifies a spatial or temporal route through which movement occurs along, via, by way of
Equalis Marks a nominal that is similar/equivalent to another; commonly

co-occurs with the verb ayuqe- "resemble"

(none)

The forms of these grammatical cases are variable, depending on the grammatical person and number of the head noun as well as the person and number of its possessor (if there is one).

Possession

Possessed nouns, like all other nouns, inflect for number and case, but also show person and number agreement with their possessor. For example, consider a few forms of saskaq "cup". The two leftmost nouns below are unpossessed, but the third is marked for a first person singular possessor -ka (pronounced in this case as -qa after assimilating to a uvular place of articulation). The final example marks plural number for both the noun itself and its possessor.

Шаблон:Interlinear

Possessors are often optional, but when present are marked with relative case:[5]

Шаблон:Interlinear

Word order

Yup'ik has considerably more freedom of word order than English does. In English, the word order of subjects and objects with respect to a verb reflects the thematic roles of the subject and object. For example, the English sentence The dog bit the preacher means something different than The preacher bit the dog does; this is because in English, the noun that comes before the verb must be the agent (the biter), while the noun following the verb must be the theme (the individual or thing that is bitten).

In Yupʼik, word order is freer because the rich inflectional system often serves to unambiguously identify thematic relations without recourse to word order. The Yup'ik sentences Qimugtem keggellrua agayulirta (dog.ERG bit preacher.ABS) and Agayulirta keggellrua qimugtem (preacher.ABS bit dog.ERG) both mean "the dog bit the preacher", for instance: the word order varies between these sentences, but the fact that qimugtem ("dog") is marked with ergative case (-m) is sufficient to identify it as the thematic agent. Thus, to say "the preacher bit the dog" in Yup'ik, one would need change which noun gets ergative case and which gets absolutive: qimugta keggellrua agayulirtem (dog.ABS bit preacher.ERG).[24]

Despite the greater freedom of word order, there seems to be a general preference for Subject-Object-Verb (SOV) order (though VSO is also common, and pragmatic factors also play a significant role).[5] This can be observed in circumstances where the inflectional system will not unambiguously determine which noun is the agent and which is the theme. This obtains, for instance, when both arguments of an indicative transitive verb are third person plural and unpossessed: elitnauristet mikelnguut assikait can, in principle, mean either "the teachers like the children" or "the children like the teachers", since the case marking on elitnauristet "teachers" and mikelnguut "children" does not distinguish ergative from absolutive case (-t marks unpossessed ergative plurals as well as unpossessed absolutive plurals). In cases like this, the SOV preference comes into play, and the sentence is most readily interpreted as "the teachers like the children".

Spatial deixis

Yup'ik has a rich system of spatial deixis. That is, many of the spatial properties of things and events are linguistically encoded in great detail; this holds true for demonstrative pronouns (like English "this one", "that one") as well as spatial adverbs ("here", "there").

There are twelve categories that define the orientation of a thing or event with respect to the environment. The environment in this sense includes topographical features (e.g., there is a contrast between upriver and downriver), the participants in the speech event (e.g., there is a contrast between proximity to the speaker and proximity to the hearer), and the linguistic context (one of these twelve categories is used for anaphora). This twelve-way contrast is cross-cut by a trinomial contrast in horizontal extension/motion: this determines whether the referent is extended (horizontally long or moving) or non-extended, and if non-extended, whether distal (typically far away, indistinct, and invisible) or proximal (typically nearby, distinct, and visible).

To illustrate, the spatial demonstrative roots of Yup'ik (which are then inflected for case and number) are presented in the following table from Miyaoka (2012).

Class Translation of class Extended Non-extended
Distal Proximal
I here (near speaker) mat- u-
II there (near hearer) tamat- tau-
III aforementioned / known im-
IV approaching (in space or time) uk-
V over there au͡g- am- ing-
VI across there, on the opposite bank ag- akm- ik-
VII back/up there, away from river pau͡g- pam- ping-
VIII up/above there (vertically) pag- pakm- pik-
IX down/below there, toward river (bank) un- cam- kan-
X out there, toward exit, downriver un'g- cakm- ug-
XI inside, upriver, inland qau͡g- qam- kiug-/kiu͡g-
XII outside, north qag- qakm- kex-

Note that Classes I and II lack distal forms due to an inherently non-distal meaning (these forms only locate things that are near to the speaker/hearer). Class III is purely anaphoric, and thus only has a distal form.[5]

Yupʼik language education

Small changes have been made towards teaching Yupʼik to the native Alaskan Yupʼiks. In 1972, the Alaska State Legislature passed legislation mandating that if "a [school is attended] by at least 15 pupils whose primary language is other than English, [then the school] shall have at least one teacher who is fluent in the native language".[25] Then, during the mid-1970s, educational programs emerged in order to revive and sustain the Yupʼik language: MacLean notes that "In 1975, an Alaska State statute was enacted directing all school boards to '...provide a bilingual-bicultural education program for each school...which is attended by at least 8 pupils of limited English-speaking ability and whose primary language is other than EnglishШаблон:'".[26] However, "the statute addressed all languages other than English, and thus expanded bilingualism equally to immigrant languages," meaning that although the statute welcomed non-English languages into schools, its primary "aim" was to "promote English proficiency", not to keep Yupʼik alive.[26]

Later, during the 1987-8 school year, three organizations, including members of the Alaska Native community, "initiated a process to establish an Alaska Native Language Policy for schools in Alaska," which "states that schools have a responsibility to teach and use as the medium of instruction the Alaska Native language of the local community to the extent desired by the parents of that community".[26] This proposal for the Alaska Native Language Policy comes three years after Steven A. Jacobson's "Central Yupʼik and the Schools: A Handbook for Teachers", a guide for teachers which exemplifies differences and similarities between English and Yupʼik so that Yupʼik or English-speaking teachers might successfully engage English-speaking Eskimo Yupʼik students in a "bilingual-bicultural education" that teaches their native language.[27]

In 2018, Anchorage's first Yup'ik immersion program was launched at College Gate Elementary.[28] Yup'ik language courses are offered at the University of Alaska Anchorage and the University of Alaska Fairbanks. The latter also offers Bachelor's degrees in Yupʼik Language and Culture, as well as associate degrees in Native Language Education with a concentration in Yupʼik, and certificates in Yupʼik Language Proficiency.[29][30]

See also

References

Шаблон:Reflist

Bibliography

External links

Шаблон:Incubator Шаблон:Wiktionary

Шаблон:Eskimo-Aleut languages Шаблон:Languages of Alaska

  1. Шаблон:Cite web
  2. Шаблон:E25
  3. Шаблон:Cite web
  4. Шаблон:Cite web
  5. 5,00 5,01 5,02 5,03 5,04 5,05 5,06 5,07 5,08 5,09 5,10 5,11 5,12 5,13 5,14 5,15 5,16 5,17 5,18 5,19 5,20 5,21 5,22 5,23 5,24 5,25 5,26 5,27 5,28 5,29 5,30 5,31 5,32 5,33 5,34 5,35 5,36 5,37 5,38 5,39 5,40 5,41 5,42 5,43 5,44 5,45 Шаблон:Cite book
  6. Шаблон:Harvnb
  7. Шаблон:Cite web
  8. 8,0 8,1 8,2 8,3 Шаблон:Harvnb
  9. 9,00 9,01 9,02 9,03 9,04 9,05 9,06 9,07 9,08 9,09 9,10 9,11 9,12 9,13 9,14 9,15 9,16 Шаблон:Cite book
  10. Шаблон:Cite book
  11. E. Irene Reed, Steven Jacobson, Lawrence Kaplan, and Jeff Leer (1985). Alaskan Eskimo Languages population, dialects, and distribution based on 1980 CensusШаблон:Dead link. Alaska Native Language Center, University of Alaska Fairbanks 1985.
  12. Шаблон:Cite webШаблон:Dead link
  13. Шаблон:Cite web
  14. 14,0 14,1 14,2 14,3 Шаблон:Cite journal
  15. 15,0 15,1 Шаблон:Cite book
  16. 16,0 16,1 Шаблон:Cite journal
  17. 17,0 17,1 Шаблон:Harvnb
  18. Шаблон:Cite book
  19. Шаблон:Cite book
  20. Шаблон:Cite book
  21. Шаблон:Citation
  22. Шаблон:Cite book
  23. Шаблон:Harvnb
  24. Шаблон:Cite web
  25. Krauss, Michael E. 1974. Alaska Native language legislation. International Journal of American Linguistics 40(2).150-52.
  26. 26,0 26,1 26,2 Шаблон:Harvnb
  27. Шаблон:Harvnb
  28. Шаблон:Cite web
  29. Шаблон:Cite web
  30. Шаблон:Cite web