Английская Википедия:Guided imagery

Материал из Онлайн справочника
Перейти к навигацииПерейти к поиску

Шаблон:Short description Шаблон:Excessive citations Guided imagery (also known as guided affective imagery, or katathym-imaginative psychotherapy) is a mind-body intervention by which a trained practitioner or teacher helps a participant or patient to evoke and generate mental images[1] that simulate or recreate the sensory perception[2][3] of sights,[4][5] sounds,[6] tastes,[7] smells,[8] movements,[9] and images associated with touch, such as texture, temperature, and pressure,[10] as well as imaginative or mental content that the participant or patient experiences as defying conventional sensory categories,[11] and that may precipitate strong emotions or feelings[12][13][14] in the absence of the stimuli to which correlating sensory receptors are receptive.[15][16]

The practitioner or teacher may facilitate this process in person to an individual or a group or you may do it with a virtual group. Alternatively, the participant or patient may follow guidance provided by a sound recording, video, or audiovisual media comprising spoken instruction that may be accompanied by music or sound.[17]

Mental imagery in everyday life

There are two fundamental ways by which mental imagery is generated: voluntary and involuntary.

The involuntary and spontaneous generation of mental images is integral to ordinary sensory perception, and cognition, and occurs without volitional intent. Meanwhile, many different aspects of everyday problem solving, scientific reasoning, and creative activity involve the volitional and deliberate generation of mental images.[18]

Involuntary

The generation of involuntary mental imagery is created directly from present sensory stimulation and perceptual information, such as when someone sees an object, creates mental images of it, and maintains this imagery as they look away or close their eyes; or when someone hears a noise and maintains an auditory image of it, after the sound ceases or is no longer perceptible.

Voluntary

Voluntary mental imagery may resemble previous sensory perception and experience, recalled from memory; or the images may be entirely novel and the product of fantasy.[19][20][21]

Technique

The term guided imagery denotes the technique used in the second (voluntary) instance, by which images are recalled from long-term or short-term memory, or created from fantasy, or a combination of both, in response to guidance, instruction, or supervision. Guided imagery is, therefore, the assisted simulation or re-creation of perceptual experience across sensory modalities.[22][23]

Clinical investigation and scientific research

Mental imagery can result from both voluntary and involuntary processes, and it comprises simulation or recreation of perceptual experience across all sensory modalities,[24] including olfactory imagery, gustatory imagery, haptic imagery, and motor imagery.[25] Nonetheless, visual and auditory mental images are reported as being the most frequently experienced by people ordinarily as well as in controlled experiments,[26] with visual imagery remaining the most extensively researched and documented in scientific literature.[27]

In experimental and cognitive psychology, researchers have concentrated primarily on voluntary and deliberately generated imagery, which the participant or patient creates, inspects, and transforms, such as by evoking imagery of an intimidating social event, and transforming the images into those indicative of a pleasant and self-affirming experience.

In psychopathology, clinicians have typically focused on involuntary imagery which "comes to mind" unbidden, such as in a depressed person's experience of intrusive unwelcome negative images indicative of sadness, hopelessness, and morbidity;[28] or images that recapitulate previous distressing events that characterize posttraumatic stress disorder.[29]

In clinical practice and psychopathology, involuntary mental images are considered intrusive when they occur unwanted and unbidden, "hijacking attention" to some extent.[30][31]

The maintenance of, or "holding in mind" imagery, whether voluntary or involuntary, places considerable demands upon cognitive attentional resources, including working memory, redirecting them away from a specific cognitive task or general-purpose concentration and toward the imagery.

In clinical practice, this process can be positively exploited therapeutically by training the participant or patient to focus attention on a significantly demanding task, which successfully competes for and directs attention away from the unbidden intrusive imagery, decreasing its intensity, vividness, and duration, and consequently alleviating distress or pain.[32][33]

Mental imagery and ill health

Mental imagery, especially visual and auditory imagery, can exacerbate and aggravate a number of mental and physical conditions.[34]

This is because, according to the principles of psychophysiology and psychoneuroimmunology, the way an individual perceives his or her mental and physical condition in turn affects biological processes, including susceptibility to illness, infection, or disease; and that perception is derived significantly from mental imagery. That is to say that in some cases, the severity of an individual's mental and physical disability, disorder, or illness is partially determined by his or her images, including their content, vividness or intensity, clarity, and frequency with which they are experienced as intrusive and unbidden.[35]

An individual can aggravate the symptoms and intensify the pain or distress precipitated by many conditions through generating, often involuntarily, mental imagery that emphasizes its severity.

For example, mental imagery has been shown to play a key role in contributing to, exacerbating, or intensifying the experience and symptoms of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD),[36] compulsive cravings,[37] eating disorders[38] such as anorexia nervosa[39] and bulimia nervosa,[40] spastic hemiplegia,[41] incapacitation following a stroke or cerebrovascular accident,[42] restricted cognitive function and motor control due to multiple sclerosis,[43] social anxiety or phobia,[44] bipolar disorder,[45] schizophrenia,[46] attention deficit hyperactivity disorder,[47] and depression.[48][49]

Example conditions aggravated by mental imagery

The aforementioned challenges and difficulties are some of those for which there is evidence to show that an individual can aggravate the symptoms and intensify the pain or distress precipitated by the condition through generating mental imagery that emphasizes its severity.

The following elaborates the way in which such mental imagery contributes to or aggravates four specific conditions:

  1. Posttraumatic stress disorder
  2. Social anxiety
  3. Depression
  4. Bipolar disorder

Posttraumatic stress disorder

Posttraumatic stress disorder often proceeds from experiencing or witnessing a traumatic event involving death, serious injury, or significant threat to others or oneself;[50] and disturbing intrusive images, often described by the patient as 'flashbacks', are a common symptom of this condition across demographics of age, gender, and the nature of the precipitating traumatic event.[51] This unbidden mental imagery is often highly vivid, and provokes memories of the original trauma, accompanied by heightened emotions or feelings and the subjective experience of danger and threat to safety in the present "here and now".[52]

Social anxiety

Individuals with social anxiety have a higher than normal tendency to fear situations that involve public attention, such as speaking to an audience or being interviewed, meeting people with whom they are unfamiliar, and attending events of an unpredictable nature.[53] As with posttraumatic stress disorder, vivid mental imagery is a common experience for those with social anxiety, and often comprises images that revive and replay a previously experienced stressful, intimidating or harrowing event that precipitated negative feelings, such as embarrassment, shame, or awkwardness.[54][55] Thereby, mental imagery contributes to the maintenance and persistence of social anxiety, as it does with posttraumatic stress disorder.[56][57]

In particular, the mental imagery commonly described by those suffering from social anxiety often comprises what cognitive psychologists describe as an "observer perspective". This consists of an image of themselves, as though from an observing person's perspective, in which those suffering from social anxiety perceive themselves negatively, as if from that observing person's point of view.[58][59] Such imagery is also common among those suffering from other types of anxiety, who often have depleted ability to generate neutral, positive, or pleasant imagery.[60]

Depression

The capacity to evoke pleasant and positively affirming imagery, either voluntarily or involuntarily, may be a critical requisite for precipitating and sustaining positive moods or feelings and optimism; and this ability is often impaired in those suffering from depression.[61] Depression consists of emotional distress and cognitive impairment that may include feelings of hopelessness, pervasive sadness, pessimism, lack of motivation, social withdrawal, difficulty in concentrating on mental or physical tasks, and disrupted sleep.[62]

Whilst depression is frequently associated with negative rumination of verbal thought patterns manifested as unspoken inner speech,[63] ninety percent of depressed patients reporting distressing intrusive mental imagery that often simulates and recollect previous negative experiences,[64][65] and which the depressed person often interprets in a way that intensifies feelings of despair and hopelessness.[66][67] In addition, people suffering from depression have difficulty in evoking prospective imagery indicative of a positive future.[68] The prospective mental imagery experienced by depressed persons when at their most despairing commonly includes vivid and graphic images related to suicide, which some psychologists and psychiatrists refer to as "flash-forwards".[69][70]

Bipolar disorder

Bipolar disorder is characterized by manic episodes interspersed with periods of depression;[71] 90% of patients experience comorbid anxiety disorder at some stage;[72] and there is a significant prevalence of suicide amongst sufferers.[73][74] Prospective mental imagery indicative of hyperactivity or mania and hopelessness contributes to the manic and depressive episodes respectively in bipolar disorder.[75]

Principles

The therapeutic use of guided imagery, as part of a multimodal treatment plan incorporating other suitable methods, such as guided meditation, receptive music therapy, and relaxation techniques, as well as physical medicine and rehabilitation,[21] and psychotherapy, aims to educate the patient in altering their mental imagery, replacing images that compound pain, recollect and reconstruct distressing events, intensify feelings of hopelessness, or reaffirm debilitation, with those that emphasize physical comfort, functional capacity, mental equanimity, and optimism.

Whether the guided imagery is provided in person by a facilitator, or delivered via media, the verbal instruction consists of words, often pre-scripted, intended to direct the participant's attention to imagined visual, auditory, tactile, gustatory or olfactory sensations that precipitate a positive psychologic and physiologic response that incorporates increased mental and physical relaxation and decreased mental and physical stress.

Guided imagery is one of the means by which therapists, teachers, or practitioners seek to achieve this outcome, and involves encouraging patients or participants to imagine alternative perspectives, thoughts, and behaviors, mentally rehearsing strategies that they may subsequently actualize, thereby developing increased coping skills and ability.[76]

Stages

According to the computational theory of imagery,[77][78][79] which is derived from experimental psychology, guided imagery comprises four phases:[80]

  1. Image generation
  2. Image maintenance
  3. Image inspection
  4. Image transformation

Image generation

Image generation involves generating mental imagery, either directly from sensory data and perceptual experience, or from memory, or from fantasy.[81]

Image maintenance

Image maintenance involves the volitional sustaining or maintaining of imagery, without which, a mental image is subject to rapid decay with an average duration of only 250 ms.[82] This is because volitionally created mental images usually fade rapidly once generated in order to avoid disrupting or confusing the process of ordinary sensory perception.[83][84][85]

The natural brief duration of mental imagery means that the active maintenance stage of guided imagery, which is necessary for the subsequent stages of inspection and transformation, requires cognitive concentration of attention by the participant. This concentrative attentional ability can be improved with the practice of mental exercises, including those derived from guided meditation and supervised meditative praxis.[86][87][88] Even with such practice, some people can struggle to maintain a mental image "clearly in mind" for more than a few seconds;[89][90][91] not only for imagery created through fantasy[92] but also for mental images generated from both long-term memory[93] and short-term memory.[94]

In addition, while the majority of the research literature has tended to focus on the maintenance of visual mental images, imagery in other sensory modalities also necessitates a volitional maintenance process in order for further inspection or transformation to be possible.[6]

The requisite for practice in sustaining attentional control, such that attention remains focused on maintaining generated imagery, is one of the reasons that guided meditation, which supports such concentration, is often integrated into the provision of guided imagery as part of the intervention. Guided meditation assists participants in extending the duration for which generated mental images are maintained, providing time to inspect the imagery, and proceed to the final transformation stage of guided imagery.[95][96]

Image inspection

Once generated and maintained, a mental image can be inspected to provide the basis for interpretation, and transformation.[97] For visual imagery, inspection often involves a scanning process, by which the participant directs attention across and around an image, simulating shifts in perceptual perspective.[98]

Inspection processes can be applied both to imagery created spontaneously, and to imagery generated in response to scripted or impromptu verbal descriptions provided by the facilitator.[99][100][101]

Image transformation

Finally, with the assistance of verbal instruction from the guided imagery practitioner or teacher, the participant transforms, modifies, or alters the content of generated mental imagery, in such a way as to substitute images that provoke negative feelings, are indicative of suffering, or that reaffirm disability or debilitation for those that elicit positive emotion, and are suggestive of resourcefulness, ability to cope, and an increased degree of mental and physical capacity.[102][103]

This process shares principles with those that inform the clinical psychology techniques of "imagery restructuring" or "imagery re-scripting" as used in cognitive behavioral therapy.[34][104][105]

While the majority of research findings on image transformation relate to visual mental imagery, there is evidence to support transformations in other sensory modalities such as auditory imagery.[6] and haptic imagery.[106]

Outcome of image generation, maintenance, inspection, and transformation

Through this technique, a patient is assisted in reducing the tendency to evoke images indicative of the distressing, painful, or debilitative nature of a condition, and learns instead to evoke mental imagery of their identity, body, and circumstances that emphasizes the capacity for autonomy and self-determination, positive proactive activity, and the ability to cope, whilst managing their condition.

As a result, symptoms become less incapacitating, pain is to some degree decreased, while coping skills increase.[107][108][109][110]

Requisite for absorption

In order for the foregoing process to take place effectively, such that all four stages of guided imagery are completed with therapeutic beneficial effect, the patient or participant must be capable of or susceptible to absorption, which is an "openness to absorbing and self-altering experiences".[111][112] This is a further reason why guided meditation or some form of meditative praxis, relaxation techniques, and meditation music or receptive music therapy are often combined with or form an integral part of the operational and practical use of the guided imagery intervention. For, all those techniques can increase the participant's or patient's capacity for or susceptibility to absorption, thereby increasing the potential efficacy of guided imagery.[113][114]

As a mind-body intervention

Шаблон:Main The United States National Center for Complementary and Integrative Health (NCCIH), which is among twenty-seven organizations that make up the National Institutes of Health (NIH), classifies guided imagery and guided meditation, as mind–body interventions, one of five domains of medical and health care systems, practices, and products that are not presently considered part of conventional medicine.[115]

The NCCIH defines mind-body interventions as those practices that "employ a variety of techniques designed to facilitate the mind's capacity to affect bodily function and symptoms", and include guided imagery, guided meditation and forms of meditative praxis, hypnosis and hypnotherapy, prayer, as well as art therapy, music therapy, and dance therapy.[116]

All mind–body interventions, including the aforementioned, focus on the interaction between the brain, body, and behavior and are practiced with intention to use the mind to alter physical function and promote overall health and wellbeing.[117][118]

There are documented benefits of mind-body interventions derived from scientific research firstly into their use in contributing to the treatment a range of conditions including headaches, coronary artery disease and chronic pain; secondly in ameliorating the symptoms of chemotherapy-induced nausea, vomiting, and localised physical pain in patients with cancer; thirdly in increasing the perceived capacity to cope with significant problems and challenges; and fourthly in improving the reported overall quality-of-life. In addition, there is evidence supporting the brain and central nervous system's influence on the immune system and the capacity for mind-body interventions to enhance immune function outcomes, including defense against and recovery from infection and disease.[119][120][121][122][123]

Guided imagery has also demonstrated efficacy in reducing postoperative discomfort as well as chronic pain related to cancer, arthritis, and physical injury.[124][125][126] Furthermore, the non-clinical uses for which the efficacy of guided imagery has been shown include managing the stress of public performance among musicians, enhancing athletic and competitive sports ability, and training medical students in surgical skills.[127] The evidence that it is effective for non-musculoskeletal pain is encouraging but not definitive.[128]

Evidence and explanation

Evidence and explanations for the effectiveness and limitations of creative visualization come from two discreet sources: cognitive psychology and psychoneuroimmunology.

Cognitive psychology

Guided imagery is employed as an adjunctive technique to psychological therapies in the treatment of many conditions, including those identified in the previous sections. It plays a significant role in the application of cognitive approaches to psychotherapy, including cognitive behavioral therapy, rational emotive behavior therapy, schema therapy, and mindfulness-based cognitive therapy.[129]

These therapies derive from or draw substantially upon a model of mental functioning initially established by Aaron T. Beck, a psychiatrist and psychoanalyst who posited that the subjective way in which people perceive themselves and interpret experiences influences their emotional, behavioral, and physiological reactions to circumstances. He additionally discovered that by assisting patients in correcting their misperceptions and misinterpretations, and aiding them in modifying unhelpful and self-deprecating ways of thinking about themselves and their predicament, his patients had more productive reactions to events, and developed a more positive self-concept, self-image, or perception of themselves.[130][131]

This use of guided imagery is based on the following premise. Everyone participates in both the voluntary and involuntary spontaneous generation of visual, auditory and other mental images, which is a necessary part of the way in which a person solves problems, recollects the past, predicts and plans the future, and formulates their self-perception, self-image, or the way they 'view' and perceive themselves.[92][132][4]

However, this self-image can be altered and self-regulated with the aid of mind-body interventions including guided imagery, by which an individual changes the way he or she visualizes, imagines, and perceives themselves generally, and their physical condition, body image, and mental state specifically.[133]

Psychoneuroimmunology

Шаблон:Main The term "psychoneuroimmunology" was coined by American psychologist Robert Ader in 1981 to describe the study of interactions between psychological, neurological, and immune systems.[134]

Three years later, Jean Achterberg published a book called Imagery in Healing that sought to relate and correlate contemporaneous evidence from the then emerging scientific study of the way mental processes influence physical and physiological function, with particular emphasis on mental imagery, to the folklore she extrapolated from a set of diverse ancient and geographically indigenous practices previously described as 'shamanism' by the historian of religion and professor at the University of Chicago, Mircea Eliade; and a number of anthropologists and ethnologists.[135][136]

The fundamental hypothesis of psychoneuroimmunology is concisely that the way people think and how they feel directly influences the electrochemistry of the brain and central nervous system, which in turn has a significant influence on the immune system and its capacity to defend the body against disease, infection, and ill health. Meanwhile, the immune system affects brain chemistry and its electrical activity, which in turn has a considerable impact on the way we think and feel.[137]

Because of this interplay, a person's negative thoughts, feelings, and perceptions, such as pessimistic predictions about the future, regretful ruminations upon the past, low self-esteem, and depleted belief in self-determination and a capacity to cope can undermine the efficiency of the immune system, increasing vulnerability to ill health. Simultaneously, the biochemical indicators of ill health monitored by the immune system feeds back to the brain via the nervous system, which exacerbates thoughts and feelings of a negative nature. That is to say, we feel and think of ourselves as unwell, which contributes to physical conditions of ill health, which in turn cause us to feel and think of ourselves as unwell.[138]

However, the interplay between cognitive and emotional, neurological, and immunological processes also provides for the possibility of positively influencing the body and enhancing physical health by changing the way we think and feel. For example, people who are able to deconstruct the cognitive distortions that precipitate perpetual pessimism and hopelessness and further develop the capacity to perceive themselves as having a significant degree of self-determination and capacity to cope are more likely to avoid and recover from ill health more quickly than those who remain engaged in negative thoughts and feelings.[139]

This simplification of a complex interaction of interrelated systems and the capacity of the mind to influence the body does not account for the significant influence that other factors have on mental and physical well-being, including exercise, diet, and social interaction.

Nonetheless, in helping people to make such changes to their habitual thought processes and pervasive feelings, mind-body interventions, including creative visualization, when provided as part of a multimodal and interdisciplinary treatment program of other methods, such as cognitive behavioral therapy, have been shown to contribute significantly to treatment of and recovery from a range of conditions.

In addition, there is evidence supporting the brain and central nervous system's influence on the immune system and the capacity for mind-body interventions to enhance immune function outcomes, including defense against and recovery from infection and disease.[140]

See also

References

Шаблон:Reflist

Шаблон:Dreaming

  1. Complementary, Alternative, or Integrative Health: What's In a Name? US Department of Health and Human Services. Public Health Service. National Institutes of Health. NIH Publication No. D347. Online Version. Retrieved 31 July 2015.
  2. Kosslyn S. M., Ganis G., and Thompson W. L., Neural foundations of imagery. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, Vol. 2, No. 9, 2001, pp. 635–642.
  3. Шаблон:Cite journal
  4. 4,0 4,1 McAvinue, L. P., and Robertson, I. H., Measuring visual imagery ability: A review. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, Vol. 26, No. 3, 2007, pp. 191–211.
  5. Cocude, M., and Denis, M., Measuring the temporal characteristics of visual images. Journal of Mental Imagery, Vol. 12, No. 1, 1988, pp. 89–101.
  6. 6,0 6,1 6,2 Zatorre, R. J., Halpern, A. R., and Bouffard, M., Mental reversal of imagined melodies: A role for the posterior parietal cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, Vol. 22, No. 4, 2010, pp. 775–789.
  7. Tiggemann, M., and Kemps, E., The phenomenology of food cravings: The role of mental imagery. Appetite, Vol. 45, No. 3, 2005, pp. 305–313.
  8. Stevenson, R. J., and Case, T. I., Olfactory imagery: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, Vol. 12, No. 2, 2005, pp. 244–264.
  9. McAvinue, L. P., and Robertson, I. H., Measuring motor imagery ability: A review. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, Vol. 20, No. 2, 2008, pp. 232–251.
  10. Juttner, M., and Rentschler, I., Imagery in multi-modal object learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, Vol. 25, No. 2, 2002, pp. 197–198.
  11. Banissy, M. J., Walsh, V., and Ward, J., Enhanced sensory perception in synesthesia. Experimental Brain Research, Vol. 196, No. 4, 2009, pp. 565–571.
  12. Lang, P. J., Levin, D. N., Miller, G. A., and Kozak, M. J., Fear behavior, fear imagery, and the psychophysiology of emotion: The problem of affective response integration. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Vol. 92, No. 3,1983, pp. 276–306.
  13. Holmes, E. A., Coughtrey, A. E., and Connor, A., Looking at or through rose-tinted glasses? Imagery perspective and positive mood. Emotion, Vol. 8, No. 6, 2008, pp. 875–879.
  14. Holmes, E. A., and Mathews, A., Mental imagery in emotion and emotional disorders. Clinical Psychology Review, Vol. 30, No. 3, 2010, pp. 349–362,
  15. Kosslyn, S. M., Ganis, G., and Thompson, W. L., Neural foundations of imagery. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, Vol. 2, No. 9, 2001, pp635–642.
  16. Lang, P. J., A Bio-Informational Theory of Emotional Imagery. Psychophysiology, Vol. 16, 1979, pp495–512.
  17. Sources:
    • Morris, C., The use of self-service technologies in stress management: A pilot project. Master of Social Work Clinical Research Papers. Saint Catherine University, St. Paul, MN, 2012.
    • Carter, E., Pre-packaged guided imagery for stress reduction: Initial results. Counselling, Psychotherapy and Health, Vol. 2, No. 2, 2006, pp. 27–39.
    • Naik, M. N. S., Effect of guided imagery on life style among alcoholics. Sinhgad e-Journal of Nursing, Vol. 11, 2013.
    • Morris, C. W., and Morris, C. D., Increasing healthy habits and health behavior change in corporate wellness programs. Corporate Wellness Programs: Linking Employee and Organizational Health, Vol. 215, 2014.
    • Meador, K. S., The effect of synectics training on gifted and non-gifted kindergarten students. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, Vol.18, 1994, pp. 55–73.
    • Meador, K. S., Fishkin, A. S., and Hoover, M., Research-based strategies and programs to facilitate creativity. In Fishkin, A. S., Cramond, B., and Olszewski-Kubilius, P. (Eds.), Investigating creativity in youth: Research and methods, pp389-415. Cresskill, NJ: Hampton,1999.
    • Carter, E., Pre-packaged guided imagery for stress reduction: Initial results. Counselling, Psychotherapy and Health, Vol. 2, No. 2, 2006, pp. 27–39.
  18. Sources:
    • Gardner, M., Creating minds. New York: Basic Books, 1953.
    • Antonietti, A., and Baldo, S., Undergraduates conceptions of cognitive functions of mental imagery. Perceptual and Motor Skills, Vol. 78, No. 1, 1994, pp. 160–162.
    • Pearson, D. G., Mental imagery and creative thought. Proceedings of the British Academy, Vol. 147, 2007, pp. 187–212.
    • Pearson, D. G., De Beni, R., and Cornoldi, C., The generation and transformation of visuo-spatial mental images. In M. Denis, R. H. Logie, C. Cornoldi, M. de Vega, and J. Engelkamp (Eds.), Imagery, language and visuo-spatial thinking. Hove: Psychology Press, 2001, pp. 1–23.
    • Shepard, R. N., and Metzler, J., Mental rotation of 3-dimensional objects. Science, Vol. 171 No. 3972, 1971, pp. 701–703.
  19. Hitch, G. J., Brandimonte, M. A., and Walker, P., Two types of representation in visual memory—Evidence from the effects of stimulus contrast on image combination. Memory and Cognition, Vol. 23, No. 2, 1995, pp. 147–154.
  20. Pearson, D. G., and Logie, R. H., Effects of stimulus modality and working memory load on mental synthesis performance. Imagination, Cognition, and Personality, Vol. 23, Nos. 2-3, 2004, pp. 183–192.
  21. 21,0 21,1 Шаблон:Cite journal
  22. Kosslyn S.M., Ganis G., Thompson W.L. Neural foundations of imagery. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. Vol. 2, No. 9, 2001, pp. 635–642.
  23. Pearson D.G. Mental imagery and creative thought. Proceedings of the British Academy. Vol. 147, 2007; pp. 187–212.
  24. Sources:
    • Kosslyn, S. M., Ganis, G., and Thompson, W. L., Neural foundations of imagery. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, Vol. 2, No. 9, 2001, pp. 635–642.
    • Pearson, D. G., Mental imagery and creative thought. Proceedings of the British Academy, Vol. 147, 2007, pp. 187–212.
  25. Sources:
    • Stevenson, R. J., and Case, T. I., Olfactory imagery: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, Vol. 12, No. 2, 2005, pp. 244–264. (olfactory imagery)
    • Tiggemann, M., and Kemps, E., The phenomenology of food cravings: The role of mental imagery. Appetite, Vol. 45, No. 3, 2005, pp. 305–313. (gustatory imagery)
    • Juttner, M., and Rentschler, I., Imagery in multi-modal object learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, Vol. 25, No. 2, 2002, pp. 197–198. (haptic imagery)
    • Holmes, P., and Calmels, C., A neuroscientific review of imagery and observation use in sport. Journal of Motor Behavior, Vol. 40, No. 5, 2008, pp. 433–445. (motor imagery)
    • Olsson, C. J., and Nyberg, L., Motor imagery: If you can't do it, you won't think it. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine and Science in Sports, Vol. 20, No. 5, 2010, pp. 711–715. (motor imagery)
    • McAvinue, L. P., and Robertson, I. H., Measuring motor imagery ability: A review. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, Vol. 20, No. 2, 2008, pp. 232–251. (motor imagery)
  26. Sources:
    • Betts, G. H., The distribution and functions of mental imagery. New York: Columbia University, 1909.
    • Tiggemann, M., and Kemps, E., The phenomenology of food cravings: The role of mental imagery. Appetite, Vol. 45, No. 3, 2005, pp. 305–313.
  27. Sources:
    • Kosslyn, S. M., Thompson, W. L., and Ganis, G., The case for mental imagery. New York: Oxford University Press, Inc., 2006.
    • Pearson, D. G., De Beni, R., and Cornoldi, C., The generation and transformation of visuo-spatial mental images. In M. Denis, R. H. Logie, C. Cornoldi, M. de Vega, and J. Engelkamp (Eds.), Imagery, language and visuo-spatial thinking. Hove: Psychology Press, 2001, pp. 1–23.
    • Logie, R. H., Visuo-spatial working memory Hove. UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1995.
  28. Sources:
    • Kosslyn, S. M., Image and mind. Harvard University Press, 1980.
    • Kosslyn, S. M., Seeing and imagining in the cerebral hemispheres: a computational approach. Psychological review, Vol. 94, No. 2, 1987, p. 148.
    • Kosslyn, S. M., Image and brain: The resolution of the imagery debate. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press, 1994.
  29. Sources:
    • Ehlers, A., and Clark, D. M., A cognitive model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, Vol. 38, No. 4, 2000, pp. 319–345.
    • Brewin, C. R., Dalgleish, T., and Joseph, S., A dual representation theory of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychological Review, Vol. 103, No. 4, 1996, pp. 670–686.
  30. Berntsen, D., Involuntary memories of emotional events: Do memories of traumas and extremely happy events differ? Applied Cognitive Psychology, Vol.15, No. 7, 2001, S135–S158.
  31. Clark, I. A., Holmes, E. A., and Mackay, C. A., Intrusive imagery and Post Traumatic Stress Disorder: An experimental psychopathology approach to flashbacks. In A. Mishara, P. Corlett, P. Fletcher and M. Schwartz (Eds.), Phenomenological neuropsychiatry: The patient's experience, bridging clinic with clinical neuroscience: Springer, 2013.
  32. Bexton, W. H., Heron, W., and Scott, T. H., Effects of decreased variation in the sensory environment. Canadian Journal of Psychology, Vol. 8, No. 2, 1954, pp. 70–76.
  33. Engelhard, I. M., van den Hout, M. A., and Smeets, M. A. M., Taxing working memory reduces vividness and emotional intensity of images about the Queen's Day tragedy. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, Vol. 42, No. 1, 2011, pp. 32–37.
  34. 34,0 34,1 Arntz, A., Imagery rescripting as a therapeutic technique: Review of clinical trials, basic studies, and research agenda. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, Vol. 3, 2012, pp. 121–126.
  35. Sources:
    • Ader, R., A historical account of conditioned immunobiologic responses. In R. Ader (Ed.), Psvchoneuroimmunology, pp. 321-352. New York: Academic Press, 1981.
    • Borysenko, J., Psychoneuroimmunology: behavioral factors and the immune response. ReVision, Vol. 7, No. 1, 1984, pp. 56-65.
    • Rossi, E., The Psychobioloqy of Mind-Body Healing. New York: W.W. Norton and Company, Inc., 1986.
  36. Sources:
    • Ehlers, A., and Clark, D. M., A cognitive model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, Vol. 38, No. 4, 2000, pp. 319–345.
    • Brewin, C. R., Dalgleish, T., and Joseph, S., A dual representation theory of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychological Review, Vol. 103, No. 4, 1996, pp. 670–686.
    • Holmes, E. A., Grey, N., and Young, K. A. D., Intrusive images and "hotspots" of trauma memories in posttraumatic stress disorder: An exploratory investigation of emotions and cognitive themes. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, Vol. 36, No. 1, 2005, pp. 3-17.
  37. May, J., Andrade, J., Panabokke, N., and Kavanagh, D., Images of desire: Cognitive models of craving. Memory, Vol. 12, No. 4, 2004, pp. 447-461.
  38. Tatham, M., The role of imagery-based techniques in cognitive–behavioral therapy for adults with eating disorders. Clinical Psychology Review, Vol. 31, No. 7, 2011, pp. 1101–1109.
  39. Guardia, D., Lafargue, G., Thomas, P., Dodin, V., Cottencin, O., and Luyat, M., Anticipation of body-scaled action is modified in anorexia nervosa. Neuropsychologia, Vol. 48, No. 13, 2010, pp3961–3966.
  40. Urgesi, C., Fornasari, L., De Faccio, S., Perini, L., Mattiussi, E., Ciano, R., and Brambilla, P., (2011). Body schema and self-representation in patients with bulimia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, Vol. 44, No. 3, 2011, pp. 238–248.
  41. Williams, J., Anderson, V., Reid, S. M., and Reddihough, D. S., Motor imagery of the un- affected hand in children with spastic hemiplegia. Developmental Neuropsychology, Vol. 37, No. 1, 2012, pp. 84–97.
  42. Sources:
    • Liepert, J., Greiner, J., Nedelko, V., and Dettmers, C., Reduced upper limb sensation impairs mental chronometry for motor imagery after stroke: Clinical and electrophysiological findings. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair, Vol. 26, No. 5,2012, pp. 470–478.
    • Nilsen, D. M., Gillen, G., DiRusso, T., and Gordon, A. M., Effect of imagery perspective on occupational performance after stroke: A randomized controlled trial. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, Vol. 66, No. 3, 2012, pp320–329.
  43. Heremans, E., Nieuwboer, A., Spildooren, J., De Bondt, S., D'hooge, A. M., Helsen, W., and Feys, P., Cued motor imagery in patients with multiple sclerosis. Neuroscience, Vol. 206, 2012, pp. 115–121.
  44. Sources:
    • Clark, D. M., and Wells, A., A cognitive model of social phobia. Social phobia: Diagnosis, assessment, and treatment, Vol. 41, No. 68, 1995, pp. 22–3.
    • Rapee, R. M., and Heimberg, R. G., A cognitive–behavioral model of anxiety in social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, Vol. 35, 1997, pp. 741–756.
    • Hackmann, A., Clark, D. M., and McManus, F., Recurrent images and early memories in social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, Vol. 38, No. 6, 2000, pp. 601–610.
    • Hirsch, C. R., Clark, D. M., and Mathews, A., Imagery and interpretations in social phobia: Support for the combined cognitive biases hypothesis. Behavior Therapy, Vol. 37, 2006, No. 3, pp. 223–236.
  45. Holmes, E. A., Geddes, J. R., Colom, F., and Goodwin, G. M., Mental imagery as an emotional amplifier: Application to bipolar disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, Vol. 46, No. 12, 2008, pp. 1251–1258.
  46. D'Argembeau, A., Raffard, S., and Van der Linden, M., Remembering the past and imagining the future in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Vol. 117, No. 1, 2008, 247–251.
  47. Abraham, A., Windmann, S., Siefen, R., Daum, I., and Gunturkun, O., Creative thinking in adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Child Neuropsychology, Vol. 12, No. 2, 2006, pp111–123.
  48. Patel, T., Brewin, C. R., Wheatley, J., Wells, A., Fisher, P., and Myers, S., Intrusive images and memories in major depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, Vol. 45, No. 11, 2007, pp. 2573–2580.
  49. Zarrinpar, A., Deldin, P., and Kosslyn, S. M., Effects of depression on sensory/motor vs. central processing in visual mental imagery. Cognition and Emotion, Vol. 20, No. 6, 2006, pp. 737–758.
  50. American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Text revision (4th ed.). Washington D.C.: American Psychiatric Association, 2000.
  51. Sources:
    • Speckens, A. M., Ehlers, A., Hackmann, A., Roths, F. A., and Clark, D. M., Intrusive memories and rumination in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder: A phenomenological comparison. Memory, Vol. 15, 2007, pp249–257.
    • Ehlers, A., Hackmann, A., and Michael, T., Intrusive re-experiencing in post-traumatic stress disorder: Phenomenology, theory, and therapy. Memory, Vol. 12, No. 4, 2004, pp. 403–415.
    • Ehlers, A., and Steil, R., Maintenance of intrusive memories in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: A cognitive approach. Behavioral and Cognitive Psychotherapy, Vol. 23, 1995, pp. 217– 249.
  52. Sources:
    • Holmes, E. A., and Mathews, A. (2010). Mental imagery in emotion and emotional disorders. Clinical Psychology Review, Vol. 30, No. 3, pp. 349–362.
    • Brewin, C. R., Dalgleish, T., and Joseph, S., A dual representation theory of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychological Review, Vol. 103, No. 4, 1996, pp. 670–686.
    • Ehlers, A., and Clark, D. M., A cognitive model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, Vol. 38, No. 4, 2000, pp. 319–345.
  53. American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Text revision (4th ed.). Washington D.C.: American Psychiatric Association, 2000.
  54. Libby, L. K., Valenti, G., Pfent, A., and Eibach, R. P. (2011). Seeing failure in your life: Imagery perspective determines whether self-esteem shapes reactions to recalled and imagined failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 101, No. 6, pp. 1157– 1173.
  55. Hackmann, A., Clark, D. M., and McManus, F. (2000). Recurrent images and early memories in social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, Vol. 38, No. 6, pp. 601–610.
  56. Clark, D. M., and Wells, A. (1995). A cognitive model of social phobia. In R. G. Heimberg, M. Liebowitz, D. Hope, and F. Schneier (Eds.), Social phobia: Diagnosis, assessment and treatment, pp69–93. New York: Guilford Press, 1995.
  57. Rapee, R. M., and Heimberg, R. G., A cognitive–behavioral model of anxiety in social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, Vol. 35, 1997, pp. 741–756.
  58. Adrian Wells, Costas Papageorgiou, The observer perspective: biased imagery in social phobia, agoraphobia, and blood/injury phobia, Behaviour Research and Therapy, Vol. 37, No. 7, July 1999, pp. 653–658,
  59. Hackmann, A., Clark, D. M., and McManus, F., Recurrent images and early memories in social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, Vol. 38, No. 6, 2000, pp. 601–610.
  60. Sources:
    • Morrison, A. S., Amir, N., and Taylor, C. T. (2011). A behavioral index of imagery ability in social anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research, Vol. 35, 2011, pp326–332.
    • Hirsch, C. R., Clark, D. M., Williams, R., Morrison, J., and Mathews, A., Interview anxiety: Taking the perspective of a confident other changes inferential processing. Behavioral and Cognitive Psychotherapy, Vol. 33, No. 1, 2005, pp. 1–12.
    • Morrison, A. S., Amir, N., and Taylor, C. T., A behavioral index of imagery ability in social anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research, Vol. 35, 2011, pp. 326–332.
    • Morrison, A. S., Amir, N., and Taylor, C. T., A behavioral index of imagery ability in social anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research, Vol. 35, 2011, pp. 326–332
    • Hackmann, A., Clark, D. M., and McManus, F., Recurrent images and early memories in social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, Vol. 38, No. 6, 2000, pp. 601–610.
    • Lockett, S. H., Hatton, J., Turner, R., Stubbins, C., Hodgekins, J., and Fowler, D., Using a semi-structured interview to explore imagery experienced during social anxiety for clients with a diagnosis of psychosis: An exploratory study conducted within an early intervention for psychosis service. Behavioral and Cognitive Psychotherapy, Vol. 40, No. 1, 2012, pp. 55–68.
  61. Holmes, E. A., Coughtrey, A. E., and Connor, A., Looking at or through rose-tinted glasses? Imagery perspective and positive mood. Emotion, Vol. 8, No. 6, 2008, pp875–879.
  62. American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Text revision (4th ed.). Washington D.C.: American Psychiatric Association, 2000.
  63. Fresco, D. M., Frankel, A. N., Mennin, D. S., Turk, C. L., and Heimberg, R. G., Distinct and overlapping features of rumination and worry: The relationship of cognitive production to negative affective states. Cognitive Therapy and Research, Vol. 26, No. 2, 2002, pp. 179–188.
  64. Birrer, E., Michael, T., and Munsch, S., Intrusive images in PTSD and in traumatized and non-traumatized depressed patients: A cross-sectional clinical study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, Vol. 45, No. 9, 2007, pp. 2053–2065.
  65. Reynolds, M., and Brewin, C. R., Intrusive cognitions, coping strategies and emotional responses in depression, post-traumatic stress disorder and a non-clinical population. Behaviour Research and Therapy, Vol. 36, No. 2, 1998, pp. 135–147.
  66. Starr, S., and Moulds, M., The role of negative interpretations of intrusive memories in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, Vol. 93, 2006, pp. 125–132.
  67. Williams, A. D., and Moulds, M. L. (2008). Negative appraisals and cognitive avoidance of intrusive memories in depression: A replication and extension. Depression and Anxiety, Vol. 25, 2008, pp. 26–33.
  68. Sources:
    • Williams, J. M. G., Ellis, N. C., Tyers, C., Healy, H., Rose, G., and MacLeod, A. K. (1996). The specificity of autobiographical memory and imageability of the future. Memory and Cognition, Vol, 24, No. 1, pp. 116–125.
    • Werner-Seidler, A., and Moulds, M. L., Autobiographical memory characteristics in depression vulnerability: Formerly depressed individuals recall less vivid positive memories. Cognition and emotion, Vol. 25, No. 6, 2011, pp. 1087–1103.
    • Morina, N., Deeprose, C., Pusowski, C., Schmid, M., and Holmes, E. A., Prospective mental imagery in patients with major depressive disorder or anxiety disorders. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, Vol. 25, No. 8, 2011, pp1032–1037.
    • Holmes, E. A., Lang, T. J., Moulds, M. L., and Steele, A. M. (2008). Prospective and positive mental imagery deficits in dysphoria. Behaviour Research and Therapy, Vol. 46, No. 8, pp. 976–981.
    • Holmes, E. A., Lang, T. J., and Deeprose, C., Mental imagery and emotion in treatment across disorders: Using the example of depression. Cognitive Behavior Therapy, Vol. 38, No. 1, 2009, pp. 21–28.
    • Cocude, M., Charlot, V., and Denis, M. (1997). Latency and duration of visual mental images in normal and depressed subjects. Journal of Mental Imagery, Vol. 21, No. 1, pp. 127–142.
    • Rogers, M. A., Bradshaw, J. L., Phillips, J. G., Chiu, E., Mileshkin, C., and Vaddadi, K., Mental rotation in unipolar major depression. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, Vol. 24, No. 1, 2002, pp. 101–106.
  69. Crane, C., Shah, D., Barnhofer, T., and Holmes, E. A. (2012). Suicidal imagery in a previously depressed community sample. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Vol. 19, No. 1, pp. 57–69.
  70. Hales, S. A., Deeprose, C., Goodwin, G. M., and Holmes, E. A., Cognitions in bipolar disorder versus unipolar depression: Imagining suicide. Bipolar Disorders, Vol. 13, Nos. 7–8, 2011, pp. 651–661.
  71. American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Text revision (4th ed.). Washington D.C.: American Psychiatric Association, 2000.
  72. Merikangas, K. R., Akiskal, H. S., Angst, J., Greenberg, P. E., Hirschfeld, R. M., Petukhova, M., et al. (2007). Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of bipolar spectrum disorder in the National Comorbity Survey replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, Vol. 64, No. 5, pp. 543–552.
  73. Balazs, J., Benazzi, F., Rihmer, Z., Rihmer, A., Akiskal, K. K., and Akiskal, H. S., The close link between suicide and (mixed) bipolar depression: Implications for suicide prevention. Journal of Affective Disorders, Vol. 91, Nos. 2–3, 2006, pp. 133–138.
  74. Hawton, K., Sutton, L., Haw, C., Sinclair, J., and Harriss, L., Suicide and attempted suicide in bipolar disorder: A systematic review of risk factors. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, Vol. 66, No. 6, 2005, pp. 693–704.
  75. Sources:
    • Holmes, E. A., Geddes, J. R., Colom, F., and Goodwin, G. M., Mental imagery as an emotional amplifier: Application to bipolar disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, Vol. 46, No. 12, 2008, pp. 1251–1258.
    • Holmes, E. A., Deeprose, C., Fairburn, C. G., Wallace-Hadrill, S. M. A., Bonsall, M. B., Geddes, J. R., et al., Mood stability versus mood instability in bipolar disorder: A possible role for emotional mental imagery. Behaviour Research and Therapy, Vol. 49, No. 10, 2011, 707–713.
    • Hales, S. A., Deeprose, C., Goodwin, G. M., and Holmes, E. A., Cognitions in bipolar disorder versus unipolar depression: Imagining suicide. Bipolar Disorders, Vol. 13, Nos. 7–8, 2011, pp. 651–661.
    • Holmes, E. A., Deeprose, C., Fairburn, C. G., Wallace-Hadrill, S. M., Bonsall, M. B., Geddes, J. R., and Goodwin, G. M., Mood stability versus mood instability in bipolar disorder: a possible role for emotional mental imagery. Behaviour Research and Therapy, Vol. 49, No. 10, 2011, pp. 707-713.
    • McCarthy-Jones, S., Knowles, R., and Rowse, G. (2012). More than words? Hypomanic personality traits, visual imagery and verbal thought in young adults. Consciousness and Cognition, Vol. 21, No. 3, pp. 1375–1381.
    • Hales, S. A., Deeprose, C., Goodwin, G. M., and Holmes, E. A. (2011). Cognitions in bipolar disorder versus unipolar depression: Imagining suicide. Bipolar Disorders, Vol. 13, Nos. 7–8, pp. 651–661.
    • Holmes, E. A., Crane, C., Fennell, M. J. V., and Williams, J. M. G., Imagery about suicide in depression—"Flash-forwards"? Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, Vol. 38, No. 4, 2007, pp. 423–434.
  76. Lang P. J., A bio-informational theory of emotional imagery. Psychophysiology, Vo.17, 1979, pp179–192.
  77. Kosslyn, S. M., Image and mind. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press, 1980.
  78. Kosslyn, S. M. (1987). Seeing and imagining in the cerebral hemispheres. A computational approach. Psychological Review, Vol. 94, No. 2, pp. 148–175.
  79. Kosslyn, S. M., Image and brain: The resolution of the imagery debate. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press, 1994.
  80. Sources:
    • Kosslyn, S. M., Image and mind. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press, 1980.
    • Kosslyn, S. M. (1987). Seeing and imagining in the cerebral hemispheres—A computational approach. Psychological Review, Vol. 94, No. 2, 1987, pp. 148–175.
    • Kosslyn, S. M., Thompson, W. L., and Ganis, G., The case for mental imagery. New York: Oxford University Press, Inc., 2006.
    • Marr, D. C., Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. New York: Freeman, 1982.
    • Kosslyn, S. M., Seeing and imagining in the cerebral hemispheres—A computational approach. Psychological Review, Vol. 94, No. 2, 1987, pp. 148–175.
    • Cichy, R. M., Heinzle, J., and Haynes, J. -D., Imagery and perception share cortical representations of content and location. Cerebral Cortex, Vol. 22, No. 2, 2012, pp. 372–380.
    • Slotnick, S. D., Thompson, W. L., and Kosslyn, S. M., Visual memory and visual mental imagery recruit common control and sensory regions of the brain. Cognitive Neuroscience, Vol. 3, No. 1, 2012, pp. 14–20.
  81. Sources:
    • Pearson, D. G., Mental imagery and creative thought. Proceedings of the British Academy, Vol. 147, 2007, pp. 187–212.
    • Kosslyn, S. M., Image and brain: The resolution of the imagery debate. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press, 1994.
    • Kosslyn, S. M., Thompson, W. L., and Ganis, G., The case for mental imagery. New York: Oxford University Press, Inc., 2006.
    • Cornoldi, C., and Rossana, D. B., Memory and imagery: A visual trace is not a mental image. In A. C. Martin, E. G. Susan, and C. Cesare (Eds.), Theories of Memory, pp87-110. Hove, UK: Psychology Press, 1998.
    • Gardini, S., Cornoldi, C., De Beni, R., and Venneri, A., Cognitive and neuronal processes involved in sequential generation of general and specific mental images. Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung, Vol. 73, No. 5, 2009, pp633–643.
    • Farah, M. J. (1988). Is visual imagery really visual? Overlooked evidence from neuro- psychology. Psychological Review, Vol. 95, No. 3, 1988, pp. 307–317.
    • Li, J., Tang, Y. -Y., Zhou, L., Yu, Q. -B., Li, S., and Sui, D. -N., EEG dynamics reflects the partial and holistic effects in mental imagery generation. Journal of Zhejiang University-Science, Vol. 11, No. 12, 2010, pp. 944–951.
    • Hitch, G. J., Brandimonte, M. A., and Walker, P., Two types of representation in vi- sual memory—Evidence from the effects of stimulus contrast on image combination. Memory and Cognition, Vol. 23, No. 2, 1995, pp. 147–154.
    • Pearson, D. G. (2007). Mental imagery and creative thought. Proceedings of the British Academy, Vol. 147, 2007, pp. 187–212.
    • Pearson, D. G., and Logie, R. H., Effects of stimulus modality and working memory load on mental synthesis performance. Imagination, Cognition, and Personality, Vol. 23, Nos. 2-3, 2004, pp. 183–192.
    • Cornoldi, C., De Beni, R., Guisberti, F., and Massironi, M. (1998). Memory and imagery: A visual trace is not a mental image. In M. Conway, S. Gathercole, and C. Cornoldi (Eds.), Theories of memory. Hove, UK: Psychology Press, pp87-110.
  82. Kosslyn, S. M., Image and brain: The resolution of the imagery debate. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press, 1994.
  83. Kosslyn, S. M., Image and mind. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press, 1980.
  84. Kosslyn, S. M., Seeing and imagining in the cerebral hemispheres: a computational approach. Psychological review, Vol. 94, No. 2, 1987, p148.
  85. Kosslyn, S. M., Thompson, W. L., and Ganis, G. (2006). The case for mental imagery. New York: Oxford University Press, Inc.
  86. Salway, A. F. S., and Logie, R. H., Visuo-spatial working memory, movement con- trol, and executive demands. British Journal of Psychology, Vol. 86, 1995, pp. 253–269.
  87. Pearson, D. G., Logie, R. H., and Green, C., Mental manipulation, visual working memory, and executive processes. Psychologische Beitrage, Vol. 38, Nos. 3-4, 1996, pp. 324–342.
  88. Logie, R. H., and Salway, A. F. S., Working memory and modes of thinking: A secondary task approach. In K. J. Gilhooly, M. Keane, R. H. Logie, and G. Erdos (Eds.), Lines of thinking: Reflections on the psychology of thought, Vol. 2, Chicester: Wiley, 1999, pp99-113.
  89. Cocude, M., and Denis, M. (1988). Measuring the temporal characteristics of visual im-ges. Journal of Mental Imagery, Vol. 12, No. 1, pp. 89–101.
  90. Cocude, M., Charlot, V., and Denis, M., Latency and duration of visual mental im- ages in normal and depressed subjects. Journal of Mental Imagery, Vol. 21, No. 1, 1997, pp. 127–142.
  91. Pazzaglia, F., and Cornoldi, C., The role of distinct components of visuo-spatial working memory in the processing of texts. Memory, Vol. 7, No. 1, 1999, pp. 19–41.
  92. 92,0 92,1 Pearson, D. G., Mental imagery and creative thought. Proceedings of the British Academy, Vol. 147, 2007, pp. 187–212.
  93. Cocude, M., and Denis, M. (1988). Measuring the temporal characteristics of visual im- ages. Journal of Mental Imagery, Vol. 12, No. 1, pp. 89–101.
  94. Watkins, M. J., Peynircioglu, Z. F., and Brems, D. J., Pictorial rehearsal. Memory and Cognition, Vol. 12, No. 6, 1984, pp. 553–557.
  95. Kosslyn, S. M., Seeing and imagining in the cerebral hemispheres: a computational approach. Psychological review, Vol. 94, No. 2, 1987, p. 148.
  96. Kosslyn, S. M., Thompson, W. L., and Ganis, G., The case for mental imagery. New York: Oxford University Press, Inc., 2006.
  97. Kosslyn, S. M., Ganis, G., and Thompson, W. L. (2001). Neural foundations of imagery. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, Vol. 2, No. 9, pp635–642.
  98. Sources:
    • Kosslyn, S. M., Image and brain: The resolution of the imagery debate. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press., 1994.
    • Kosslyn, S. M., Thompson, W. L., and Ganis, G., The case for mental imagery. New York: Oxford University Press, Inc., 2006.
    • Denis, M., and Kosslyn, S. M., Scanning visual mental images: A window on the mind. Cahiers De Psychologie Cognitive-Current Psychology of Cognition, Vol. 18, No. 4, 1999, pp. 409–465.
    • Baddeley, A. D., Human memory: Theory and practice. Needham Heights, MA, US: Allyn and Bacon, 1990.
    • Denis, M., and Carfantan, M., Enhancing people's knowledge about images. In P. J. Hampson, D. F. Marks, and J. T. E. Richardson (Eds.), Imagery: Current developments, pp. 197–222. London: Routledge, 1990.
    • Pylyshyn, Z. W., The imagery debate. Analog media versus tacit knowledge. Psychological Review, Vol. 88, No. 1, 1981, pp. 16–45.
  99. Pearson, D. G., De Beni, R., and Cornoldi, C., The generation and transformation of visuo-spatial mental images. In M. Denis, R. H. Logie, C. Cornoldi, M. de Vega, and J. Engelkamp (Eds.), Imagery, language and visuo-spatial thinking, pp. 1–23. Hove: Psychology Press, 2001.
  100. Mellet, E., Bricogne, S., Crivello, F., Mazoyer, B., Denis, M., and Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Neural basis of mental scanning of a topographic representation built from a text. Cerebral Cortex, Vol. 12, No. 12, 2002, pp. 1322–1330.
  101. Iachini, T., and Ruggiero, G. (2010). The role of visual experience in mental scanning of actual pathways: Evidence from blind and sighted people. Perception, Vol. 39, No. 7, pp. 953–969.
  102. Sources:
    • Pearson, D. G., De Beni, R., and Cornoldi, C., The generation and transformation of visuo-spatial mental images. In M. Denis, R. H. Logie, C. Cornoldi, M. de Vega, and J. Engelkamp (Eds.), Imagery, language and visuo-spatial thinking, pp1–23. Hove: Psychology Press, 2001.
    • Shepard, R. N., and Cooper, L. A., Mental images and their transformations. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press, 1982.
  103. Kosslyn, S. M., Thompson, W. L., and Ganis, G., The case for mental imagery. New York: Oxford University Press, Inc., 2006.
    • Finke, R. A., Pinker, S., and Farah, M. J., Reinterpreting visual patterns in mental imagery. Cognitive Science, Vol. 13, No.1, 1989, pp. 51–78.
    • Verstijnen, I. M., van Leeuwen, C., Goldschmidt, G., Hamel, R., and Hennessey, J. M., Creative discovery in imagery and perception: Combining is relatively easy, restructuring takes a sketch. Acta Psychologica, Vol. 99, No. 2, 1998, pp. 177–200.
    • Verstijnen, I. M., van Leeuwen, C., Goldschmidt, G., Hamel, R., and Hennessey, J. M., Creative discovery in imagery and perception: Combining is relatively easy, restructuring takes a sketch. Acta Psychologica, Vol. 99, No. 2, 1998, pp. 177–200.
    • Reisberg, D., The nonambiguity of mental images. In C. Cornoldi, R. H. Logie, M. A. Brandimonte, G. Kaufmann, and D. Reisberg (Eds.), Stretching the imagination: Representation and transformation in mental imagery New York: Oxford University Press, 1996.
    • Verstijnen, I. M., Hennessy, J. M., van Leeuwen, C., Hamel, R., and Goldschmidt, G., Sketching and creative discovery. Design studies, Vol. 19, No. 4, 1998, pp. 519– 546.
    • Reisberg, D., and Logie, R. H. (1993). The ins and outs of working memory. In Intons-Peterson, M, Roskos-Ewoldsen, B., Blake, R., and Clayton, K. (Eds.), Imagery, creativity and discovery Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum Associates, 1993, pp. 39–76.
    • Brandimonte, M. A., and Collina, S., Verbal overshadowing in visual imagery is due to recoding interference. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, Vol. 20, No. 3, 2008, pp612–631.
    • Watkins, M. J., and Schiano, D. J., Chromatic imaging—An effect of mental coloring on recognition memory. Canadian Journal of Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie, Vol. 36, No. 2, 1982, pp. 291–299.
    • Reed, S. K., Imagery and Discovery. In Roskos-Ewoldsen, B,. Intons- Peterson, M. J., and Anderson, R. (Eds.), Imagery, creativity and discovery: A cognitive perspective Amsterdam: North-Holland, 1993.
  104. Hagenaars, M. A., Brewin, C. R., van Minnen, A., Holmes, E. A., and Hoogduin, K. A. L. (2010). Intrusive images and intrusive thoughts as different phenomena: Two experimental studies. Memory, Vol. 18, No. 1, pp. 76–84.
  105. Holmes, E. A., Arntz, A., and Smucker, M. R., Imagery rescripting in cognitive behaviour therapy: Images, treatment techniques and outcomes. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, Vol. 38, No. 4, 2007, pp. 297–305.
  106. Miquée, A., Xerri, C., Rainville, C., Anton, J. L., Nazarian, B., Roth, M., and Zennou-Azogui, Y., Neuronal substrates of haptic shape encoding and matching: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroscience, Vol. 152, No. 1,2008, pp. 29–39.
  107. van den Hout, M. A., Engelhard, I. M., Beetsma, D., Slofstra, C., Hornsveld, H., Houtveen, J., and Leer, A., EMDR and mindfulness. Eye movements and attentional breathing tax working memory and reduce vividness and emotionality of aversive ideation. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, Vol. 42, No. 4, 2011, pp423–431
  108. Holmes, E. A., Arntz, A., and Smucker, M. R., Imagery rescripting in cognitive behavior therapy: Images, treatment techniques and outcomes. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, Vol. 38, No. 4, 2007, pp. 297–305.
  109. Giesen-Bloo, J., Van Dyck, R., Spinhoven, P., Van Tilburg, W., Dirksen, C., Van Asselt, T., and Arntz, A., Outpatient psychotherapy for borderline personality disorder: randomized trial of schema-focused therapy vs transference-focused psychotherapy. Archives of General Psychiatry, Vol. 63, No. 6, 2006, pp. 649–658.
  110. Holmes, E. A., Mathews, A., Dalgleish, T., and Mackintosh, B., Positive interpretation training: Effects of mental imagery versus verbal training on positive mood. Behavior Therapy, Vol. 37, No. 3, 2006, pp. 237–247.
  111. Tellegen, A., and Atkinson, G., Openness to absorbing and self-altering experiences (absorption), a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Vol. 83, No. 3, 1974, pp. 268–277.
  112. Tellegen, A., A brief manual for the differential personality questionnaire. Minneapolis: Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, 1982.
  113. Bond, K., Ospina, M. B., Hooton, N., Bialy, L., Dryden, D. M., Buscemi, N., Shannahoff-Khalsa, D., Dusek, J., and Carlson, L. E., 'Defining a complex intervention: The development of demarcation criteria for "meditation"'. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality, Vol. 1, No. 2, May 2009, pp. 129–137.
  114. Shapiro, D. H. Jnr., 'Overview: Clinical and physiological comparison of meditation with other self-control strategies'. In Shapiro, D.H Jnr. and Walsh, R.N. (Eds.) Meditation: Classic and Contemporary Perspectives. Piscataway, New Jersey: Aldine Transaction, 1984, pp. 5–12.
  115. US National Library of Medicine. National Institutes of Health Collection Development Manual. Complementary and Alternative Medicine. 8 October 2003. Online Version. Retrieved 31 July 2015.
  116. Straus, S. E., Expanding Horizons of Healthcare: Five Year Strategic Plan 2001–2005. 25 September 2000. US Department of Health and Human Services. Public Health Service. National Institutes of Health. NIH Publication No. 01-5001. Online Version Retrieved 31 July 2015.
  117. Elkins, G., Fisher, W., and Johnson, A., Mind–body therapies in integrative oncology. In Current Treatment Options in Oncology, Vol. 11, Nos. 3-4, 2010, pp. 128–140.
  118. Wieland, L.S., Manheimer E., Berman B.M., Development and classification of an operational definition of complementary and alternative medicine for the Cochrane Collaboration. Alternative Therapies in Health and Medicine, Vol. 17, No. 2, 2011, pp. 50–59.
  119. Ernst, E., Pittler, M.H., Wider, B., and Boddy, K., Mind-body therapies: are the trial data getting stronger? Alternative Therapy in Health and Medicine, Vol. 13, No. 5, 2007, pp. 62–64.
  120. Wahbeh H., Elsas, S. M., Oken, B.S., Mind-Body Interventions: applications in neurology. Neurology, Vol. 70, No. 24, 2008, pp. 2321–2328.
  121. Mundy, E.A,. DuHamel, K.N., Montgomery, G. H., The efficacy of behavioral interventions for cancer treatment-related side effects. Seminars in Clinincal Neuropsychiatry, Vol. 8, No. 4, 2003, pp253–275.
  122. Astin, J. A., Shapiro, S. L., Eisenberg, D. M., and Forys, K. L., Mind-body medicine: state of the science, implications for practice. Journal of the American Board of Family Practice, Vol. 16, No. 2, 2003, pp. 131–147.
  123. Irwin, M. R., Human psychoneuroimmunology: 20 years of discovery. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, Vol. 22, No. 2, 2008, pp. 129–139.
  124. Schaffer, S. D., and Yucha, C. B., Relaxation and Pain Management: The relaxation response can play a role in managing chronic and acute pain. American Journal of Nursing, Vol. 104, No. 8, 2004, pp. 75–82.
  125. Eller, L. S., Guided imagery interventions for symptom management. Annual Review of Nursing Research, Vol.17, No.1, 1999, pp. 57–84.
  126. Шаблон:Cite journal
  127. Van Kuiken, D., A meta-analysis of the effect of guided imagery practice on outcomes. Journal of Holistic Nursing, Vol. 22, No. 2, 2004, pp. 164–179.
  128. Шаблон:Cite journal
  129. Hart, J., Guided imagery. Alternative and complementary therapies, Vol. 14, No. 6, 2008, pp. 295–299.
  130. Sources:
    • Beck, A. T., Thinking and depression: Theory and therapy. Archives of General Psychiatry, Vol. 10, 1964, pp. 561–571.
    • Beck, A. T., Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New York: International Universities Press, 1976.
    • Beck, A. T., The current state of cognitive therapy: A 40-year retrospective. Archives of General Psychiatry, Vol. 63, 2005, pp. 953–959.
    • Beck, A. T., The evolution of the cognitive model of depression and its neurobiological correlates. American Journal of Psychiatry, Vol.165, 2008, pp. 969–977.
    • Beck, A. T., Freeman, A., Davis, D. D., Pretzer, J., Fleming, B., and Beck, J. S., Cognitive therapy of personality disorders (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press, 2004.
    • Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., and Emery G., Cognitive therapy of depression. New York: Guilford Press, 1979.
    • Beck, J. S., Cognitive therapy: Basics and beyond. New York: Guilford Press, 1995.
  131. Beck, J. S., Cognitive therapy for challenging problems. New York: Guilford Press, 2005.
  132. White, K., Sheehan, P. W., and Ashton, R., Imagery assessment: A survey of self-report measures. Journal of Mental Imagery, Vol. 1, No. 1, 1977, pp145–169.
  133. Sources:
    • Lang, P. J., Levin, D. N., Miller, G. A., and Kozak, M. J., Fear behavior, fear imagery, and the psychophysiology of emotion: The problem of affective response integration. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Vol. 92, No. 3, 1983, pp. 276–306.
    • Pitman, R. K., Sanders, K. M., Zusman, R. M., Healy, A. R., Cheema, F., Lasko, N. B., and Orr, S. P., Pilot study of secondary prevention of posttraumatic stress disorder with propranolol. Biological psychiatry, Vol. 51, No. 2, 2002, 2002, pp. 189–192.
    • Shin, L. M., Kosslyn, S. M., McNally, R. J., Alpert, N. M., Thompson, W. L., Rauch, S. L., and Pitman, R. K., Visual imagery and perception in posttraumatic stress disorder: a positron emission tomographic investigation. Archives of General Psychiatry, Vol. 54, No. 3, 1997, pp. 233–241.
    • Rauch, S. L., van der Kolk, B. A., Fisler, R. E., Alpert, N. M., Orr, S. P., Savage, C. R., and Pitman, R. K., A symptom provocation study of posttraumatic stress disorder using positron emission tomography and script-driven imagery. Archives of General Psychiatry, Vol. 53, No. 5, 1996, pp. 380–387.
    • Lanius, R. A., Williamson, P. C., Densmore, M., Boksman, K., Gupta, M. A., Neufeld, R. W., and Menon, R. S., Neural correlates of traumatic memories in posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional MRI investigation. American Journal of Psychiatry, Vol. 158, No. 11, 2001, pp. 1920–1922.
    • Lanius, R. A., Williamson, P. C., Boksman, K., Densmore, M., Gupta, M., Neufeld, R. W., and Menon, R. S., Brain activation during script-driven imagery induced dissociative responses in PTSD: a functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation. Biological psychiatry, Vol. 52, No. 4, 2002, pp. 305–311.
  134. Ader, R., A historical account of conditioned immunobiologic responses. In R. Ader (Ed.), Psvchoneuroimmunology, pp. 321–352. New York: Academic Press, 1981.
  135. Achterburg, J., Imagery in Healing; Shamanism and Modern Medicine. Boston: New Science Library, 1985.
  136. Shamanism: Archaic Techniques of Ecstasy, Princeton University Press, Princeton, 2004
  137. Borysenko, J., Psychoneuroimmunology: behavioral factors and the immune response. ReVision, Vol. 7, No. 1, 1984, pp. 56–65.
  138. Borysenko, J., Minding the Body, Mending the Mind. MA: Addison-Wesley, 1987.
  139. Rossi, E., The Psychobioloqy of Mind-Body Healing. New York: W.W. Norton and Company, Inc., 1986.
  140. Sources:
    • Ernst, E., Pittler, M.H., Wider, B., and Boddy, K., Mind-body therapies: are the trial data getting stronger? Alternative Therapy in Health and Medicine, Vol. 13, No. 5, 2007, pp. 62–64.
    • Rutledge, J.C., Hyson, D.A., Garduno, D., Cort, D.A., Paumer, L., and Kappagoda, C. T., Lifestyle modification program in management of patients with coronary artery disease: the clinical experience in a tertiary care hospital. Journal of Cardiopulmonary Rehabilitation, Vol. 19, No. 4, 1999, pp. 226–234.
    • Wahbeh H., Elsas, S. M., Oken, B.S., Mind-Body Interventions: applications in neurology. Neurology, Vol. 70, No. 24, 2008, pp2321–2328.
    • Rutledge, J.C., Hyson, D.A., Garduno, D., Cort, D. A, Paumer, L., and Kappagoda, C. T., Lifestyle modification program in management of patients with coronary artery disease: the clinical experience in a tertiary care hospital. Journal of Cardiopulmonary Rehabilitation Vol. 19, No. 4, 1999, pp. 226–234.
    • Mundy, E.A,. DuHamel, K.N., Montgomery, G. H., The efficacy of behavioral interventions for cancer treatment-related side effects. Seminars in Clinincal Neuropsychiatry, Vol. 8, No. 4, 2003, pp. 253–275.
    • Astin, J. A., Shapiro, S. L., Eisenberg, D. M., and Forys, K. L., Mind-body medicine: state of the science, implications for practice. Journal of the American Board of Family Practice, Vol. 16, No. 2, 2003, pp. 131–147.
    • Irwin, M. R., Human psychoneuroimmunology: 20 years of discovery. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, Vol. 22, No. 2, 2008, pp. 129–139.
    • Ader, R., and Cohen, N., Behaviorally conditioned immunosuppression. Psychosomatic Medicine, Vol. 37, No. 4, 1975, pp. 333–340.